The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
INFLUENTIAL PUBLICATIONFull Access

Validating the Diagnosis of Delirium and Evaluating Its Association With Deterioration Over a One-Year Period

Published Online:

Abstract

The authors probed the associations between clinical diagnoses and independent research measures of cognitive, behavioral, and electroencephalographic (EEG) changes in hospitalized older patients and investigated the contribution of medical illness to deterioration. Patients (N=96; 47 of whom were hospitalized during the course of 1 year; 12 diagnosed with delirium) received tests of cognitive and physical functioning and the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale, specific neuropsychological tests, and a two channel EEG. Delirium was associated with independent measures of cognitive decline and EEG slowing. Hospitalization was associated with deterioration in functional status during the year, whether or not patients showed delirium. Results suggest that medical illness leading to hospitalization can contribute significantly to deterioration in self-care, and, when it is associated with delirium, to deterioration in cognitive performance and cerebral activity over a period of 1 year.

General medical conditions can have multiple effects on cognitive performance in vulnerable older patients. Acutely, they can lead to delirium and, possibly, other metabolic encephalopathies. Although there is a growing body of knowledge on the prevalence of delirium in medical inpatients, risk factors, and methods for case identification (16) questions remain about what signs and symptoms are the best indicators of the presence of toxic or metabolic encephalopathies and about long-term prognosis. Medical illness can also lead to more chronic forms of cognitive deterioration through vascular mechanisms or other paths. Evidence that it contributes to cognitive decline comes from the finding that patients with Alzheimer’s disease with general health problems exhibit significantly greater rates of decline than others (7) from an older literature on “terminal drop”—cognitive decrements observed in elderly patients shortly before death (8, 9)—and from more recent findings that the negative outcomes of late-life delirium include residual cognitive and functional deficits as well as increased mortality (1013). Addressing these issues, we report here on a prospective cohort study of elderly patients living in a residential care setting. Patients were monitored repeatedly during a period of approximately 1 year to evaluate the nature of the cognitive changes that occurred during acute medical illnesses requiring hospitalization and to assess the characteristics and correlates of the cognitive decrements observed over the course of the year.

Methods

Procedures

The findings reported here were derived from a longitudinal study of delirium and related symptoms in a residential care population. Descriptions of methods and other findings have been reported by the authors here (42) as well as elsewhere (14, 15). Consenting subjects who met the criteria for inclusion in the study were followed over 1 year, receiving assessments at baseline and at 4-, 8-, and 12-month follow-up sessions, with additional assessments during hospitalizations and after discharge. Baseline and follow-up assessments were identical, including tests of cognitive, physical, and affective functioning. In addition to assessments given at 4-month intervals, the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) was repeated the second and third week after the baseline and follow-up assessments. Those who were hospitalized at some point during this study received clinical evaluations to establish the presence or absence of definite or possible DSM-III-R delirium within 48 hours of admission, at Day 6, and every 4–5 days thereafter, as previously described. Note that DSM-III-R criteria were utilized because they were the best validated criteria available at the time that this study was designed. Also, patients received research assessments with full evaluations (including the MMSE, Buschke Selective Reminding Tests, Stroop Test, Verbal Vigilance, and electroencephalogram [EEG]) or brief evaluations (including the MMSE and measures of accessibility) given on alternate days. They also received full evaluations 2, 4, and 8 weeks after discharge from the hospital. For those who were hospitalized toward the end of the planned 1-year period of observation, additional assessments were conducted, extending the period of observation until at least 2 months after hospital discharge.

Sample

The resident population consisted of those living in a nursing home and a high-rise congregate apartment complex. Medical records were reviewed to restrict the subjects considered for participation to a group of elderly persons who did not demonstrate severe cognitive impairment and were medically stable. Subsequently, they were evaluated for inclusion/exclusion criteria. Subjects were eligible if they spoke English and had no severe visual or hearing impairment. They had to have a score of less than 17 on the Blessed Memory-Information-Concentration Test; no acute medical illness, current psychosis, or major depression; no medication change in the 2 weeks before the initial assessment; and no hospitalizations in the last month. After accounting for these criteria, 102 subjects were included in this study; 51 were from a nursing home and 51 from a congregate housing facility. To be included in the analyses of change over time reported here, subjects needed to have been assessed at two or more time points at least 3 months apart; six subjects who did not meet these criteria were omitted from these analyses, leaving 96 subjects. During the course of the year, 47 of the 96 subjects (49%) were hospitalized.

Measures

Physical health was evaluated at admission to the study by the subjects’ physicians, using the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale (CIRS) (16) to assess the severity of physical health problems in 13 (non-psychiatric) systems; a comorbidity index was computed by counting the number with moderate-to-severe pathology. The CIRS has been validated as an indicator of health status among medical and psychiatric populations (17, 18) and, more specifically, for use with this population of frail, institutionalized older adults (19).

The MMSE (20) was used as a measure of global cognitive performance. Physical functioning was measured with the Physical Self Maintenance Scale (PSMS) (21), using data from the subjects’ self-report or staff reports. The eight items were scored on a three-point scale, totaling between 8 and 24, with higher scores indicative of greater impairment. Also, a series of specific neuropsychological tests was administered. The Stroop Test (2227) is a measure of cognitive inhibition, flexibility, and mental control. Scores included the number of errors in the final condition with incongruent colors and words, and the interference score. The Buschke Selective Reminding Test (SRT) (2833) assesses verbal list learning and memory. The number of words the subject recalled in six trials is added to obtain a measure of Total Recall. Verbal Vigilance is a measure of auditory vigilance and sustained concentration. The double-letter task requires the subject to respond to a target letter only when it follows a second specified letter. Scoring counted the total correct out of 10 trials.

EEG measures. A portable, microprocessor-based, two-channel EEG device was used to measure EEG (Neurotrac, Interspec, Inc.; Conshohocken, PA). This machine provides output in the form of fast-Fourier–analyzed EEG. The method used identified data contaminated by muscle movement and electromyogram activity, the alpha squeak phenomenon, and drowsiness. Recordings were obtained from parieto-occipital leads (P3–O1, P4–O2), because background slowing, primarily in posterior leads, is a classic hallmark of delirium (34). Subjects were asked to close and open their eyes 12 times. Data were taken from the first two 2-second epochs of artifact-free EEG after eyes were closed, totaling 20 epochs or 40 seconds. Data are reported in terms of percent power in standard delta (0–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), and alpha (8–13 Hz) EEG bands. Measurements were conducted by research nurses who were trained in techniques by an EEG technician. The entire procedure took 15 to 20 minutes. This method of obtaining EEG data was previously shown to be reliable and appears to be a valid way of detecting changes in cerebral state in elderly subjects (14).

Repeated measures. In light of repeated testing, 12 different variations of the MMSE, Stroop Task, Verbal Vigilance, and Buschke SRT were given to limit potential learning effects. The 12 forms of the MMSE varied according to the list of three words used to test short-term memory, the word used to test attention, and the verbal three-step command. The 12 forms of the Stroop varied the order of stimulus presentation; the Buschke SRT provides 12 different word lists; and Verbal Vigilance varied in the target letters and order of presentation. Each subject entered in the study was given different forms initially and was rotated through the 12 forms in sequential order, starting over when given all 12 forms. In all, 1,197 MMSE measurements were obtained on 96 subjects, 486–560 other cognitive measures on 88–96 subjects; and 491 EEG measures on 90 subjects. For in-hospital data, there were 162 MMSE measures on 46 subjects; 72–76 other cognitive measures on 36–38 subjects; and 77 EEG measures on 38 subjects.

Other assessments. Other measures included self-ratings of depression conducted throughout the period of observation, and nurses’ ratings of behavioral disturbances conducted during hospitalizations. Findings will be reported elsewhere.

Analyses

Several different types of analyses were performed to evaluate changes over time for each cognitive, EEG, clinical, and functional status measure over 1 year and to make comparisons of these changes with respect to hospitalization, delirium, and baseline cognitive status. First, we assessed changes across time outside of the hospital and compared these changes between hospitalized and non-hospitalized subjects and between subjects with and without delirium. Second, we evaluated changes across time during hospitalization and non-hospitalization periods, making comparisons between these two contexts and between delirium and non-delirium patients. Third, we evaluated how changes across time differed between cognitively impaired and intact patients as ascertained at baseline.

These three types of analyses were performed with mixed-effects linear regression models (35) consisting of random intercepts and slopes across time for each individual. This model is an appropriate method to analyze repeated measures over time in studies such as this, where there are multiple repeated measures for the same subjects and when there are unequal numbers of assessments across subjects depending on whether or not they were hospitalized, and the extent to which there are missing data (36, 37). Limitations of this model include effects of certain types of non-random attrition, which are assessed, and insensitivity to nonlinear change. The models used for these analyses are presented in Appendix 1.

We supplemented these mixed model-based analyses with analogous mixed models adjusting for baseline age and CIRS. These baseline variables did not differ significantly statistically or clinically between subjects with and without delirium at baseline. The respective means and standard deviations were 84.5±6.4 and 86.1±5.9 for age and 1.70±0.32 and 1.72±0.63 for CIRS. Consequently, their inclusion in the above models only marginally altered the presented estimates, standard errors, confidence intervals (CIs), and P values for delirium comparisons under the models without these covariates. Similar robustness to adjustments for age and CIRS were observed for comparisons between hospitalized and non-hospitalized status.

For each dependent variable (cognitive, clinical, EEG, and functional measures), we obtained an estimate of each of the above effects (e.g., difference in mean at baseline, difference in slopes, significance of a slope) accompanied with 95% confidence intervals, t-statistics, and corresponding P values. All t-tests reported in the article for slopes and intercepts are based on the mixed effects models; t-tests comparing delirium and non-delirium patients may lack power because of the small number of delirium patients (12). However, basing these t-tests on the mixed models that incorporate all data collected across time for each subject improves the power, as the effective sample size is greater than the number of subjects but less than the total number of data-points collected for an outcome.

To assess the sensitivity of our results to non-random attrition related to the missing data (i.e., informative drop-out), we reanalyzed the data with the above models augmented with additional effects for linear and quadratic time to drop-out. Such models represent pattern-mixture models that serve as approximations to selection models (38). The resulting estimates were very similar to those obtained under the previous models, suggesting that the inference in this article is not affected significantly by non-random attrition.

We also considered the extent of intraindividual variability in study measures by use of measures described in Appendix 1. In order to determine whether baseline global cognitive status predicted variability in the above measures, subjects who were considered cognitively intact (MMSE≥24) were compared, by means of t-tests with respect to the variability measures, with the cognitively impaired subjects (<24) at baseline. Also, the relationship between baseline medical status (as measured by CIRS) and each variability measure was estimated with a multiple-regression analysis, with the variability measure as the dependent variable. The significance of this relationship was determined after adjusting for baseline age, gender, and education. Subjects who were hospitalized during the study were compared with those who were not with respect to variability scores for all measures described above by including the covariate for hospital status in the multiple-regression models outlined above. Also, hospitalized subjects who were diagnosed with delirium and those who were not were compared with respect to variability of each of the measures, including EEG measures, by including a new covariate in each of the above models. Subjects were included in some comparison analyses but not others because they completed some measures, such as the MMSE, but were unable to complete other measures over the year for multiple reasons.

Results

The study sample considered in these analyses consisted of a subset of 96 of the 102 subjects from the parent study for whom at least two assessments were available over a period of at least 3 months to support the analysis of change over time. The average age was 84.7; 66% were female. At admission to the study, the average MMSE score of the study sample was 24.1±4.4; range: 11–30; 60% of the subjects were cognitively intact, with MMSE of ≥24; 21% reported no limitations in basic activities of daily living, and 28% reported a limitation in only one; 47 of the subjects (49%) were hospitalized during the course of the study, and 12 of them (25.5%) were found to have definite or possible delirium during hospitalization on the basis of psychiatric evaluations.

Those patients who were hospitalized during the course of this study exhibited higher baseline measures of medical illness burden relative to the rest of the sample, but they did not differ in other baseline measures (Table 1). Those who were diagnosed with delirium exhibited higher baseline impairments in executive functioning on the Stroop test relative to the rest of those who were hospitalized, but they did not differ significantly on the MMSE or other baseline measures, including age and CIRS.

Change and variability during the course of the study outside the hospital

The changes exhibited by study subjects during the course of the year of follow-up can be characterized with the mixed-model slopes calculated from all available data obtained outside of the hospital (Table 2). For the sample as a whole, there were significant declines during the course of the year in functional status (PSMS scores) and in EEG measures (increases in % delta and % theta, decreases in % alpha power). There were increases over the year in correct responses on the Stroop test that can be attributed to practice or learning effects; these will not be discussed further. In subgroup analyses, the PSMS and EEG slopes were significantly different from zero in the patients who were hospitalized during the year, but not in those who were not. No other changes were significant in either the hospitalized or non-hospitalized subgroups. Among those who were hospitalized, changes in PSMS were significant both in those patients who were diagnosed with delirium and in those who were not. EEG slopes were significantly different from zero only in those patients who were found to show delirium during their hospitalization. In addition to those measures that exhibited changes over time in the entire study sample, MMSE variables exhibited significant changes over time, specifically in the subgroup of patients who were diagnosed with delirium when they were in the hospital.

However, these analyses reported on whether the mixed-model regression slopes were significantly different from zero in relevant subgroups. In interpreting the differences between the hospitalized and non-hospitalized patients or those between the delirium and nondelirium patients, it is also important to test for significant differences in slopes between subgroups. There were no significant differences between those subjects who were hospitalized and those who were not. Among those who were hospitalized, there was a significant difference in slopes on the MMSE between those with and without delirium (t=2.49; P=0.013), but no differences in the PSMS or in the EEG measures. There were significant differences between hospitalized and non-hospitalized subjects in their variability over time outside the hospital in the PSMS and in EEG measures (% theta, % alpha). Among those who were hospitalized, there were differences in variability between those with or without diagnoses of delirium in the PSMS but not in other measures (Table 3).

When subjects classified as cognitively intact on the basis of baseline MMSE scores were compared with those who were impaired, there were no significant differences in the proportion of subjects who were hospitalized or in the proportion of those hospitalized who were diagnosed with delirium. Those with cognitive impairment at baseline exhibited greater decline over the course of the year in the measure of verbal vigilance (t=3.76; P=0.0003) but not in other cognitive, clinical, or EEG measures. They exhibited greater variability over time in MMSE measures (t=4.20; P=0.001), interference on the Stroop test (t=2.64; P=0.01), and verbal vigilance (t=4.97; P=0.0001). There was a significant relationship between baseline CIRS scores and variability in MMSE scores (R2change=0.0416; P=0.035) even after controlling for baseline MMSE, age, gender, and education; no other associations with measures of variability were significant.

Comparison of changes during the course of the study between data for hospitalized and non-hospitalized subjects

Data acquired in the hospital were merged with the previously presented non-hospital data in a series of analyses that allowed comparison of changes in slopes and intercepts for data acquired during hospitalization with other data acquired while patients were in the long-term care setting. The slope over time for the hospitalization data did not differ significantly from that obtained outside of the hospital for any of the variables considered. The decline for hospitalization data for those diagnosed with delirium was steeper than that for all non-delirium patients for the MMSE (−4.261; 95% CI: −8.346 — −0.177 vs. −0.130; 95% CI: −0.781 — 0.520; P=0.048), but not for other variables.

The zero time intercept for hospital data differed from that for non-hospital data in measures of verbal memory, verbal vigilance, and in EEG measures (% delta, % theta, and % alpha; Table 4). The intercept for hospital data in patients diagnosed with delirium differed from that for others only for the EEG measures.

Another series of analyses compared hospital and non-hospital data after controlling for the differences that could be attributed to diagnosed delirium. Analyses of slopes demonstrated that differences for the MMSE scores remained significant (difference= −1.718; 95% CI: −3.357 — −0.079; P=0.040) after controlling for the effects of delirium. Evaluations of intercepts demonstrated that differences for verbal memory and verbal vigilance remained significant, but the EEG effects were no longer present.

Discussion

This article reports on a prospective study of delirium conducted in a long-term care facility and its associated hospitals in which subjects were followed for 1 year in their place of residence, during any hospitalizations that occurred during the year, and after discharge for the remainder of the year and until at least 2 months after the index hospitalization. The study was designed to use findings on changes observed during hospitalization and over the course of a 1-year period of observation to probe the symptomatology of delirium and its association with the patients’ clinical course.

For this study, the diagnosis of delirium was made through repeated clinical assessments conducted by subspecialty-trained geriatric psychiatrists on all hospitalized patients the second and sixth day of the hospitalization. This strategy was used to maximize the specificity of the process of case identification. We recognize, however, that cases with short-lived symptoms occurring between clinical evaluations may have been missed and that this may have limited the sensitivity of case identification. However, the study design, with its frequent research assessments, allows us to draw conclusions about the symptoms associated with these putative cases. To the extent to which these ephemeral cases occur, the group of hospitalized patients without diagnosed delirium may have been “contaminated” with delirium patients, and the analyses reported here may have underestimated the impact of delirium on the patients’ longer-term clinical course.

Previous reports from this research support the validity of these assessments by demonstrating that those patients diagnosed with delirium exhibited increased mortality relative to the remainder of those who were hospitalized; the effect size for the hazard function in Cox models was 2.24 (95% CI: 1.02–4.91). It also confirmed the validity of hospitalization as a medically significant event by demonstrating that those who were hospitalized exhibited increased mortality relative to those who were not; the effect size was 2.81 (95% CI: 1.43–5.54). As discussed below, the diagnosis of delirium is validated by the findings reported here that clinical diagnoses are associated with group differences in the slope of MMSE change during hospitalization and the intercept for EEG measures.

Antecedents and symptoms of delirium

In this sample, patients with a diagnosis of delirium differed from the remainder of the hospitalized patients in their pre-hospital baseline performance on the Stroop test, but not in their MMSE. This study did not replicate findings from previous research demonstrating that preexisting cognitive impairment was a risk factor for delirium. This result may have followed from our exclusion of patients with severe dementia from the study and from the diagnostic heterogeneity of those with cognitive impairment. Alternatively, it may reflect the low power of this study as a result of both the limited overall sample size and the small number of patients diagnosed with delirium. The findings on the Stroop test suggest that specific executive-function deficits may be a risk factor for delirium. Although this could be of conceptual importance, the relevant analyses must be considered exploratory, and the finding must be viewed with caution.

Research data acquired during hospitalizations on patients with delirium demonstrate significant differences in the MMSE slope and intercepts for EEG measures. Patients with delirium experienced greater decline in global cognition, on average, during their hospitalization. The differences in intercepts for the EEG variables indicate increased levels of delta and theta activity and decreased alpha activity in patients who were diagnosed with delirium during hospitalization. Given that our design ensured the independence of clinical and research assessments, these findings confirm current understanding of delirium as a disorder associated with global deficits in cognitive performance as well as its association with background slowing on the EEG.

To evaluate the extent to which clinical features of delirium may have occurred in patients who did not receive clinical diagnoses, we conducted a series of analyses that tested for differences between hospital and non-hospital data after controlling for the differences that were associated with the diagnosis of delirium. The EEG changes associated with hospitalization are accounted for by those patients who received clinical diagnoses. However, findings from the regression analyses demonstrate differences in MMSE slope and in intercepts for measures of verbal memory and verbal vigilance that remained significant after controlling for diagnoses of delirium. These findings suggest that although the clinical strategy used in the study for the identification of delirium was validated in terms of its association with increased mortality, it did not identify all of the individuals who experienced cognitive changes associated with hospitalization. The observation of MMSE, verbal memory, and attentional deficits that were not associated with clinical diagnoses could reflect either transient cases of delirium that appeared and resolved between clinical assessments or the existence of subclinical cases not captured by DSM-III-R diagnostic criteria. Although our clinical assessments did not capture all of the cognitive decrements associated with hospitalization, these findings demonstrate that frequent screening for the elements of delirium should identify such changes.

According to the research design, clinical evaluations were conducted to probe for the diagnosis of delirium only when patients were in the hospital. We recognized that delirium could also occur in the long-term care settings and that prevalence estimates of 6%–7% have been reported in the nursing home (39). It was our assumption that the extensive clinical services available in the facility that housed these studies would be adequate to identify these cases and that they would be captured in our design because they would, in general, lead to hospitalization. However, we have no tests for the validity of this assumption. In fact, findings on the variability of MMSE and EEG measures (see below) suggest that ephemeral, unrecognized episodes of delirium may have occurred in the long-term care facility as well as in the hospital.

Changes over the period of one year

The sample, as a whole, deteriorated over the course of the year in self-care ability as measured with the PSMS. Interestingly, these changes were significant only for those who were hospitalized during the year, whether or not they were diagnosed with delirium. This finding is consistent with a growing body of data suggesting that an identifiable subgroup of older patients are at risk for deterioration after an acute hospitalization (40, 41) and demonstrates that a major component of the functional decline that occurs in frail elderly patients is associated with significant medical events that require hospital admission. It suggests that increasing the intensity of medical care of older patients with chronic illness might serve a role in preventing deterioration.

No significant decrements in cognitive performance were found over the course of this study in this study sample as a whole. Although cognitive deterioration is a characteristic of Alzheimer’s disease, the analyses conducted identified greater decline in those patients who were classified as cognitively impaired at baseline only in tests of vigilance. In interpreting these findings, it is important to recall that each of the regression analyses controlled for baseline measures on the dependent variable. Therefore, the lack of effects may have reflected the nature of the analyses conducted as well as the heterogeneity of the subjects with baseline impairment, limitations in statistical power, or learning and practice effects related to the multiple repeated administrations of the test instruments. We did, however, observe significant within-subject decrements over the course of the year in MMSE score for those patients who were both hospitalized and diagnosed with delirium. Moreover, among those who were hospitalized, there were significant differences in the MMSE slopes for non-hospital data between those who experienced delirium and those who did not. These findings suggest that the combination of medical events and cognitive vulnerability that leads to delirium is a strong predictor of cognitive decline. Medical events and the vulnerability of the brain to the effects of medical illness, more than cognitive impairment itself, were associated with deterioration over time.

These conclusions are also consistent with the EEG findings reported here. The patient sample, as a whole, deteriorated over the course of the year, with increases in delta and theta and decreases in alpha power. This effect, however, could be attributed to significant declines only in those patients who were both hospitalized and diagnosed with delirium. Taken together, these data demonstrate that patients who experience delirium are at specific risk for declines in cognitive performance and cerebral activity during the course of a 1-year period. They cannot, however, distinguish between models in which the deterioration is a sequel or consequence of the delirium vs. those in which both delirium and deterioration occur because patients exhibit a combination of medical illness and a cerebral vulnerability to the physiological effects of illness. Even so, the EEG findings, like those on cognitive functioning, raise the possibility that medical care directed toward preventing acute illnesses might prevent deterioration in frail elderly patients.

The statistical models used in these analyses allow the quantification of within-subject variability as well as changes over time. Understanding the subjects’ variability in functional and cognitive measures could be clinically significant because fluctuations over time are characteristic of delirium and because such variability may imply that subjects are exhibiting excess disability some of the time. In fact, the observed within-subject variability in study measures may have a number of causes. Some of the associations may reflect limitations in the linear model used to characterize change over time. Others, such as the association of global cognitive impairment with increased variability in specific cognitive measures, could reflect limitations in the reliability of the instruments in specific populations. However, the findings on the association of hospitalization with greater variability in self-care during course of the year once more supports the importance of medical illness as a determinant of functional status in elderly persons. The associations of variability in MMSE scores with baseline measures of medical illness and those of variability in EEG measures with hospitalization suggest that medical vulnerability may be associated with variability in cerebral activity. These findings lead to the hypothesis that cerebral activity in frail older individuals may be sensitive to day-to-day variations in medical status.

To summarize, the findings from this prospective cohort study add to the existing body of evidence that confirms the validity of the clinical criteria used to diagnose delirium. They demonstrate that significant medical events leading to hospitalization are important markers for functional decline in elderly patients. Moreover, they demonstrate that either the occurrence of delirium or the combination of general medical and cerebral vulnerability that predispose individuals to it are important markers for global cognitive decline. Our findings demonstrate that a clinical diagnosis of delirium during a hospitalization has long-term significance as an index of frailty and a marker for a high risk for deterioration. Furthermore, several of our findings suggest that delirium can exist along a continuum in terms of its severity and persistence. One type of milder or more time-limited delirium may have been demonstrated through the finding that hospitalization was accompanied by cognitive decrements that could not be attributed to clinically diagnosed episodes of delirium. The existence of another type was suggested by the findings that subjects with greater medical illness burden at baseline and those who required hospitalization exhibited greater variability in (non-hospital) MMSE scores and EEG measures, respectively. Thus, for frail elderly patients, delirium appears to represent a spectrum of conditions ranging from those that are highly significant complications of acute illnesses to those that are part of the psychopathology of everyday life.

Appendix 1. Statistical Models
We first present the model for assessing changes across hospitalized and non-hospitalized time in one of the response variables (cognitive, EEG, clinical, and functional measures), and then incorporate delirium status into the model. Alterations to the model to assess differences between hospitalized and non-hospitalized patients with respect to changes across time spent outside the hospital are addressed, as is investigation of the effect of baseline cognitive impairment on changes across time.
First, denoting fixed-effects scalar parameters with “B,” random-effects scalar parameters with “b,” and independent scalar variables with capital variables, the mixed model for assessing the effect of hospitalization on change across time may be written as:
Yij = (B0 + b0i) + (B1 + b1i)Tij + (B2 + b2i)Hij + (B3 + b3i)Tij Hij + B4 BYi + eij, where
Yij = The response variable at the jth time point for the ith subject
    B0 Fixed-effects intercept for non-hospitalized observations of each subject at the zero time point
    b0i Random-effects intercept for the non-hospitalized observations of the ith subject
    B1 Fixed-effects slope parameter for non-hospitalized observations of each subject
    b1i Random-effects slope parameter for the non-hospitalized observations of the ith subject
    Tijjth time point for the ith subject, where Ti1 = 0 corresponds to the time of enrollment into the study.
    Hij Hospitalization status at the jth time point of the ith subject (1 = hospitalized, 0 = non-hospitalized)
    B2 Difference in fixed-effects intercept between non-hospitalized and hospitalized observations for the ith subject, if hospitalized
    b2i Difference in random-effects intercept between non-hospitalized and hospitalized observations for the ith subject, if hospitalized
    B3 Difference in fixed-effects slope parameter between non-hospitalized and hospitalized observations for the ith subject, if hospitalized
    b3i Difference in random-effects slope parameter between non-hospitalized and hospitalized observations for the ith subject, if hospitalized
    B4 Fixed-effects slope parameter for the baseline response for each subject
    BYi Baseline response for the ith subject
    eij Measurement or statistical error for the jth time-point observation of the ith subject
This model gives two sets of estimates of change over time, fixed and random effects. Each of the fixed effects represents an average for the entire sample, whereas the random effects represent individual deviations from the sample average. More specifically, the fixed effects intercept B0 represents the baseline mean response level for subjects who were not hospitalized at baseline. The fixed-effects slope B1 represents the mean slope for subjects in the non-hospitalized period. The fixed-effects parameter B2 represents the mean difference between hospitalized and non-hospitalized observations. The fixed-effect parameter B3 represents the difference in mean slopes between hospitalized and non-hospitalized observations. The random effects are assumed to have a normal distribution and allow the model to account for the correlation or lack of independence between observations from the same individual. The model includes a parameter that represents the effects of the baseline outcome (BYij), so all of the above effects are adjusted for baseline outcome. Another set of mixed-model analyses were conducted to probe for differences in regression slopes and intercepts between patients who were clinically diagnosed with delirium and the remainder of the data obtained on the study sample. For these analyses, the above model used for the hospitalized vs. non-hospitalized comparisons was augmented with the following terms:
B5 Di + (B6 + b6i)TijDi where Di = delirium status of the ith subject (1 = delirium, 0 = non-delirium)
    B5 + Fixed-effects difference in intercept between non-delirium and delirium patients at time zero
    B6 + Difference in fixed-effects slope for time between non-delirium and delirium patients
    b6i + Difference in random-effects slope between non-delirium and delirium patients.
The fixed-effects intercept B0 and slope B1 are now interpreted as before, but for the non-delirium patients. The parameters B2 and B3 are interpreted as before in terms of differences in means and slopes between hospitalized and non-hospitalized status, but adjusting for delirium status. The new parameter B5 represents the difference in mean responses levels between non-delirium and delirium subjects at the start of the study adjusted for hospital status. The parameter B6 represents the difference in mean slope for time between non-delirium and delirium subjects, also adjusting for hospital status. Sub-group analyses of slopes and intercepts for combinations of delirium and hospitalization status were performed by including interaction terms between hospitalization, delirium, and time. In addition, comparisons between cognitively intact and non-intact patients were performed by replacing the delirium indicator (Di) in the above model with cognitively-intact indicator for the ith subject. Similarly, comparisons between non-hospitalized subjects with the post-hospitalization observations of formerly hospitalized subjects were performed by replacing the time-varying indicator of hospitalization status (Hij) with a baseline indicator of hospitalization status that remains constant across time for each subject.
We were also interested in looking at intra-individual variability in study measures obtained outside of the hospital. This was calculated by the following:
    where= sum across all time points indexed by j and observed for a given subject
    v = variability of performance for the ith subject
    peij expected score calculated from the random effects model at the jth time point for the ith subject
    poij observed score at the jth time point for the ith subject
    ni number of data points for the ith subject
This score estimates the variability of all obtained scores for each subject on a measure around the random-effect slope for that measure.
When comparing groups of subjects in terms of averages of this variability measure, these variability scores were weighted on the basis of the number of data-points obtained in order to account for the difference in variance of the variability scores.
Table 1. Baseline Characteristics of Study Subjects
  Total SampleSubgroups
 MeanDifference (H vs. NH)t-score (H vs. NH)Difference (D vs. ND)t-score (D vs. ND)
Age, years84.71.71.310.70.30
Education, years10.9−0.7−0.84−0.4−0.31
CIRS2.60.92.30.10.17
PSMS10.70.81.251.81.70
MMSE24.1−0.9−1.02−1.8−1.23
Stroop,
    % correct79.1−2.1−0.5416.93.17
    Interference3.50.10.210.20.23
Verbal memory25.8−2.3−1.63−3.4−1.62
Verbal vigilance8.90.10.38−0.02−0.43
EEG
    Delta0.060.0070.050.010.12
    Theta0.190.0150.350.020.35
    Alpha0.460.0200.320.050.32

Note: H vs. NH=non-hospital data for patients who were hospitalized vs. others; D vs. ND=non-hospital data for patients who were diagnosed with delirium vs. other hospitalized patients.

Entries with P<0.05 are in bold.

Table 1. Baseline Characteristics of Study Subjects
Enlarge table
Table 2. Slopes for Non-hospital Data
 Total SampleHospitalized PatientsDelirium Patients  
 Mean95% CIMean95% CIMean95% CIt(H vs. NH)t(D vs. ND)
PSMS1.322(0.577, 2.068)2.007(0.974, 3.039)3.350(0.951, 5.750)−1.81−1.27
MMSE0.058(−0.481, 0.596)0.072(−0.707, 0.850)−2.396(−4.553, −0.239)−0.062.49
Stroop
    % correct3.440(0.300, 6.580)3.170(−1.400, 7.700)4.850(−7.800, 17.500)0.17−0.38
    Interference−0.486(−1.081, 0.109)−0.496(−1.349, 0.347)0.047(−2.105, 2.199)0.00−0.48
Verbal memory−0.385(−1.058, 1.828)−0.393(−2.385, 1.599)−0.447(−4.642, 3.747)1.100.06
Verbal vigilance0.115(−0.0537, 0.306)−0.281(−0.850, 0.288)−0.277(−1.643, 1.088)0.850.00
EEG
    Delta0.018(0.006, 0.029)0.022(0.006, 0.038)0.045(0.012, 0.078)−0.76−1.54
    Theta0.033(0.010, 0.055)0.041(0.010, 0.072)0.092(0.020, 0.165)−0.72−1.55
    Alpha−0.042(−0.070, −0.014)−0.051(−0.090, −0.012)−0.099(−0.189, −0.008)0.691.18

Note: H vs. NH=non-hospital data for patients who were hospitalized vs. others; D vs. ND=non-hospital data for patients who were diagnosed with delirium vs. other hospitalized patients; PSMS=Physical Self-Maintainance Scale; MMSE=Mini-Mental State Exam.

Cells with P<0.05 are in bold.

Table 2. Slopes for Non-hospital Data
Enlarge table
Table 3. Variability Over Time (t Scores for Differences)
 (H vs. NH)(D vs. ND)
PSMS−2.54−3.38
MMSE−0.74−0.79
Stroop
    % correct−1.41−0.44
    Interference−0.82−0.23
Verbal memory1.280.52
Verbal vigilance−0.59−1.62
EEG
    Delta−1.47−1.96
    Theta−2.26−1.76
    Alpha−3.44−0.38

Note: H vs. NH=non-hospital data for patients who were hospitalized vs. others; D vs. ND=non-hospital data for patients who were diagnosed with delirium vs. other hospitalized patients; PSMS=Physical Self-Maintainance Scale; MMSE=Mini-Mental State Exam.

Cells with P<0.05 are in bold.

Table 3. Variability Over Time (t Scores for Differences)
Enlarge table
Table 4. Differences in “Zero Time” Intercepts
 Between Hospital and Non-Hospital DataBetween Delirium and Non-Delirium DataBetween Hospital and Non-Hospital Data (controlling for delirium)
VariableDifferenceCIPDifferenceCIPDifferenceCIP
MMSE−0.507(−1.412, 0.398)0.265−0.202(−2.086, 1.682)0.816−0.250(−1.315, 0.815)0.639
Stroop
    % correct−1.297(−5.741, 3.147)0.556−2.994(−14.663, 8.675)0.553−1.547(−6.459, 3.365)0.524
    Interference0.007(−0.719, 0.734)0.9840.673(−1.311, 2.657)0.4230.004(−0.795, 0.804)0.991
Buschke SRT3.419(5.546,1.291)0.002−3.611(−8.232, 1.011)0.1093.174(5.559,0.790)0.011
Verbal vigilance1.419(2.186,0.651)0.001−1.546(−3.114, 0.022)0.0531.115(1.968,0.262)0.012
Delta activity
    right hemisphere0.034(0.015, 0.052)0.0010.055(0.022, 0.088)0.0050.019(−0.002, 0.040)0.079
Theta activity
    right hemisphere0.055(0.022, 0.088)0.0020.116(0.062, 0.170)0.0010.031(−0.005, 0.067)0.091
Alpha activity
    right hemisphere0.058(0.095,0.020)0.0040.097(0.160,0.034)0.007−0.030(−0.072, 0.012)0.156

Note: CI=95% confidence interval; MMSE=Mini-Mental State Exam; SRT=Selective Reminding Test.

Cells with P<0.05 are in bold.

Table 4. Differences in “Zero Time” Intercepts
Enlarge table

(Reprinted with permission from the American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 2001; 9:148–159)

References

1 American Psychiatric Association: Practice guideline for the treatment of patients with delirium. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156(suppl 5):1–20CrossrefGoogle Scholar

2 Levkoff S, Liptzin B, Cleary P, et al: Review of research instruments and techniques used to detect delirium. Int Psychogeriatr 1991; 3:253–271CrossrefGoogle Scholar

3 Inouye SK, van Dyck CH, Alessi CA, et al: Clarifying confusion: the confusion assessment method: a new method for detection of delirium. Ann Intern Med 1990; 113:941–948CrossrefGoogle Scholar

4 Johnson JC, Kerse NM, Gottlieb G, et al: Prospective vs. retrospective methods of identifying patients with delirium. J Am Geriatr Soc 1992; 40:316–319CrossrefGoogle Scholar

5 Rolfson DB, McElhaney JE, Rockwood K, et al: Incidence and risk factors for delirium and other adverse outcomes in older adults after coronary artery bypass graft surgery. Can J Cardiol 1999; 15:771–776Google Scholar

6 Inouye SK, Bogardus ST Jr, Charpentier PA, et al: A multicomponent intervention to prevent delirium in hospitalized older patients. New Engl J Med 1999; 340:669–676CrossrefGoogle Scholar

7 Teri L, Hughes JP, Larson EB: Cognitive deterioration in Alzheimer’s disease: behavioral and health factors. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 1990; 45:P58–P63CrossrefGoogle Scholar

8 Johansson B, Zarit SH: Early cognitive markers of the incidence of dementia and mortality: a longitudinal population-based study of the oldest old. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1997; 12:53–59CrossrefGoogle Scholar

9 Johansson B, Berg S: The robustness of the terminal decline phenomenon: longitudinal data from the Digit-Span Memory Test. J Gerontol 1989; 44:P184–P186CrossrefGoogle Scholar

10 Murray AM, Levkoff SE, Wetle TT, et al: Acute delirium and functional decline in the hospitalized elderly patient. J Gerontol 1993; 48:M181–M186CrossrefGoogle Scholar

11 Francis J, Kapoor WN: Prognosis after hospital discharge of older medical patients with delirium. J Am Geriatr Soc 1992; 40:601–606CrossrefGoogle Scholar

12 Levkoff SE, Evans DA, Liptzin B, et al: Delirium: the occurrence and persistence of symptoms among elderly hospitalized patients. Arch Intern Med 1992; 152:334–340CrossrefGoogle Scholar

13 Inouye SK, Rushing JT, Foreman MD, et al: Does delirium contribute to poor hospital outcomes? J Gen Intern Med 1998; 13:234–242CrossrefGoogle Scholar

14 Katz IR, Mossey J, Sussman N, et al: Bedside clinical and electrophysiological assessment: assessment of change in vulnerable patients. Int Psychogeriatr 1991; 3:289–300CrossrefGoogle Scholar

15 Katz IR, Sussman NM, Mossey J, et al: Utilization of a two-channel, microprocessor-based EEG device for monitoring cognitively vulnerable patients. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1990; 3:155–162CrossrefGoogle Scholar

16 Linn BS, Linn MW, Gurel L: Cumulative Illness Rating Scale. J Am Geriatr Soc 1968; 16:622–626CrossrefGoogle Scholar

17 Conwell Y, Forbes NT, Cox C, et al: Validation of a measure of physical illness burden, at autopsy: the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale. J Am Geriatr Soc 1993; 41:38–41CrossrefGoogle Scholar

18 Miller MD, Paradis CF, Houck PR, et al: Rating chronic medical illness in burden in geropsychiatric practice and research: application of the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale. Psychiatry Res 1991; 41:237–248CrossrefGoogle Scholar

19 Parmelee PA, Thuras PD, Katz IR, et al: Validation of the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale in a geriatric residential population. J Am Geriatr Soc 1995; 43:130–137CrossrefGoogle Scholar

20 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: “Mini-Mental State:” a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189–198CrossrefGoogle Scholar

21 Lawton MP Brody EM: Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist 1969; 9:179–188CrossrefGoogle Scholar

22 Stroop JR: Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935; 28:643–661CrossrefGoogle Scholar

23 MacLeod CM: Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol Bull 1991; 109:163–203CrossrefGoogle Scholar

24 Graf P, Uttl B, Tuokko H: Color- and picture-word Stroop tests: performance changes in old age. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1995; 17:390–415CrossrefGoogle Scholar

25 Ivnik RJ, Malec JF, Smith GE, et al: Neuropsychological tests’ norms above age 55: COWAT, BNT, MAE token, WRAT-R reading, AMNART, STROOP, TMT, and JLO. Clin Neuropsychol 1996; 10:262–278CrossrefGoogle Scholar

26 Spreen O, Strauss E: A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. New York, Oxford University Press, 1991Google Scholar

27 Uttl B, Graf P: Color-word Stroop test performance across the adult life span. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1997; 19:405–420CrossrefGoogle Scholar

28 Buschke H, Fuld PA: Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurology 1974; 11:1019–1025CrossrefGoogle Scholar

29 Masur DM, Sliwinski M, Lipton RB, et al: Neuropsychological prediction of dementia and the absence of dementia in healthy elderly persons. Neurology 1974; 44:1427–1432CrossrefGoogle Scholar

30 Paulsen JS, Salmon DP, Monsch AU, et al: Discrimination of cortical from subcortical dementias on the basis of memory and problem-solving tests. J Clin Psychol 1995; 51:48–58CrossrefGoogle Scholar

31 Katz IR, Sands LP, Bilker W, et al: Identification of medications that cause cognitive impairment in older people: the case of oxybutynin chloride. J Am Geriatr Soc 1998; 46:8–13CrossrefGoogle Scholar

32 Ray PG, Meador KJ, Loring DW, et al: Central anticholinergic hypersensitivity in aging. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1992; 5:72–77CrossrefGoogle Scholar

33 Zemishlany Z, Thorne AE: Anticholinergic challenge and cognitive functions: a comparison between young and elderly normal subjects. Israel J Psychiatry Related Sci 1991; 28:32–41Google Scholar

34 Engel G, Romano J: Delirium: a syndrome of cerebral insufficiency. J Chron Dis 1959; 9:260–277CrossrefGoogle Scholar

35 Laird NM, Ware JH: Random-effects models for longitudinal data. Biometrics 1982; 38:963–974CrossrefGoogle Scholar

36 Brandt LJ, German PS, Rovner BW, et al: A longitudinal approach to modeling outcomes in a nursing home study. Gerontologist 1992; 32:159–163CrossrefGoogle Scholar

37 German PS, Rovner BW, Burton LC, et al: The role of mental morbidity in the nursing home experience. Gerontologist 1992; 32:152–158CrossrefGoogle Scholar

38 Wu M, Follmann D: Use of summary measures to adjust for informative missingness in repeated-measures data with random effects. Biometrics 1995; 55:75–84CrossrefGoogle Scholar

39 Rovner BW, German PS, Broadhead J, et al: The prevalence and management of dementia and other psychiatric disorders in nursing homes. Int Psychogeriatr 1990; 2:13–24CrossrefGoogle Scholar

40 Sager MA, Franke T, Inouye SK, et al: Functional outcomes of acute medical illness and hospitalization in older persons. Arch Intern Med 1996; 156:645–652CrossrefGoogle Scholar

41 Hansen K, Mahoney J, Palta M: Risk factors for lack of recovery of ADL independence after hospital discharge. J Am Geriatr Soc 1999; 47:360–365CrossrefGoogle Scholar

42 Curyto KJ, Johnson J, TenHave T, et al: Survival of hospitalized elderly patients: a prospective study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2001; 9:141–147CrossrefGoogle Scholar