The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Influential PublicationsFull Access

How to Understand Divergent Views on Bipolar Disorder in Youth

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.focus.140105

Abstract

There are two divergent viewpoints on the phenomenology and outcome of bipolar I (BP I) disorder in youth. Disparities evolved as unintended consequences from investigators’ inconsistencies both in translating the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-III, DSM-III-R, and DSM-IV criteria and in operationalizing them differently in their standardized assessments. Rates of conservatively diagnosed BP I are lower both in community studies of youths than in adults and from liberally defined BP I in youths. Rates of co-occurring attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are lower in conservatively than liberally defined children and adolescents with BP I. Rates of both BP I and of ADHD are lower in off-spring of BP I probands, and outcome more closely approximates that of adults with BP I in conservatively versus liberally defined children and teens with BP I. Both perspectives can claim evidence for reliability and validity that support their positions. However, the samples are so different that it is difficult to compare studies conducted from these different perspectives.

(Reprinted from Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 2014; 10:529–551, with permission from Annual Reviews)

History of the Controversy Over the Diagnosis of Mania/Bipolar Disorder in Youth

The nature of mania, and therefore bipolar I (BP I) disorder, in children (versus adolescents) has been a cause of debate for many years. It was most clearly outlined in a “point-counterpoint” between Biederman and Gittleman-Klein in 1998 (Biederman et al. 1998), but others have addressed the question before (Carlson 1990) and since (Galanter & Leibenluft 2008, Harrington & Myatt 2003, Kent & Craddock 2003). The debate has even extended to adults (Wingo & Ghaemi 2007). The core of the disagreement was and remains whether mania in children should be diagnosed narrowly, with discrete episodes that represent a clear departure from the person’s prior function and usual state with symptoms that are the same as one observes for adults (the conservative or narrow viewpoint), or if it should be diagnosed more broadly (the liberal or broad viewpoint). The broader definition appears to embody early childhood onset of chronic emotion dysregulation, usually superimposed on symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional defiant disorder with multiple other comorbidities. It includes severe irritability manifested by intense and protracted rage outbursts (Mick et al. 2005), a “bipolar phenotype” as measured by the Child Behavior Checklist (Biederman et al. 1995), as well as by modified manic symptoms as described in the Washington University version of the Schedule of Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for Children (WASH-U-KSADS; Geller et al. 2001). Episodes are extremely protracted. The cases meet the letter of the mania criteria identified in the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-III to DSM-IV-R (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1980, 1987, 1994, 2000) as measured by the investigators. The question is whether children with this condition have a complicated, very early onset form of adult bipolar disorder or experience mood problems concomitant with their many other comorbidities (Perlis et al. 2004).

After the National Institute of Mental Health’s Bipolar Roundtable in 2000 (Natl. Inst. Ment. Health 2001), some investigators started to describe the different mania/bipolar phenomenologies as narrow and broad phenotypes. Although some researchers focused on the importance of elation and grandiosity as central to the narrow phenotype (Geller et al. 2002), others identified discrete episodes as the primary distinguishing feature (Carlson 1990, Leibenluft et al. 2003). This second approach is the one used in this critical review. That is, the narrow or conservative view of mania/BP I is identified by the clarity of episodes; the broad or liberal view by manic symptoms without well-defined episodicity.

Disagreement about the boundaries of mania in children is not a new problem, as both mania and childhood have a variety of definitions (Carlson & Glovinsky 2009). The German physician and psychologist, Theodor Ziehen, in 1917 defined mania as

pathological mirth… joined by heightened anger. In severe cases, fierce outbursts of rage may follow. The accelerated association of ideas, the so-called flight of ideas, manifests itself in incessant, quick speech, the so-called logorrhea. Frequently, the child does not let parents nor teachers get a word in, butts in during class time and rambles from one subject to another… troubled sleep (agrypnia) stands in direct relation with the accelerated movement compulsion and accelerated association of ideas… delusive ideas occur. In many cases, they merely show themselves as a shift of the entire imaginative contents in terms of heightened ego. The shift leads to a behavioral norm of loud ostentation, precociousness, rebellion against the parental and instructive authority, fantastic plans for the future and the like. (as cited in Carlson & Glovinsky 2009, p. 261)

Ziehen’s rather classical description, however, applied to children older than age 11. What differentiated mania in children from the behaviors of typical young children was the fact that the symptoms reflected a rapid change from the child’s characteristic behavior.

Clinical interest in manic-depression in youth began in greater earnest after Kraepelin’s (1921) monograph on manic-depressive insanity in the early twentieth century. Surveys of mentally ill youths, culminating in a special issue of the journal The Nervous Child in 1952 (e.g., Barton-Hall 1952, Harms 1952), concluded that depression was more common than mania and occurred mostly in adolescents but that there may be “temperamental variants in children” (for a review, see Carlson & Glovinsky 2009). Anthony & Scott (1960), however, applied a conservative view to a literature search for mania in children, and requiring strictly defined, classical manic-depressive symptoms in preadolescents, they found the condition to be rare.

Ironically, the introduction of explicit criteria for the definition of psychiatric disorders in general, and mania and depression in particular (Feighner et al. 1972), laid the foundation for a broader view of pediatric bipolar disorder. Inspired by the Feighner criteria, Weinberg & Brumback (1976), two pediatric neurologists from Washington University, systematically examined the children in their learning disabilities clinic and identified a number as having mania rather than being hyperkinetic, the DSM-II (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1968) name for ADHD.

The Emergence of Two Views of Mania/Bipolar Disorder

Changes in Conceptualizations of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Oppositional Behavior

It is clear that considerable overlap exists between symptoms of ADHD and mania. The question is why, in recent years, has there been such an increased focus on these cases (Blader & Carlson 2007, Moreno et al. 2007), and how has this impacted our understanding of mania? Reasons emerge from decisions made since 1980 in the DSM criteria both for what previously was called “hyperkinetic reaction of childhood” (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1968) and for mania/bipolar disorder.

Hyperactive children, diagnosed prior to the DSM-III (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1980) as having hyperkinetic reaction of childhood, have a condition that we still recognize. Hyperactivity is the most striking symptom.

This may be noted from early infancy on or not become prominent until five or six years of age. There are also a short attention span and poor powers of concentration, which are particularly noticeable under school conditions. (Laufer et al. 1957, p. 38)

However, struck from the DSM-III (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1980) and subsequent iterations are what appear to be mood symptoms and behavioral fluctuations seen in many hyperactive children. “Variability also is frequent, with the child being described as quite unpredictable and with wide fluctuations in performance. Outstandingly also these children seem unable to tolerate any delay in gratification of their needs and demands. They are irritable and explosive, with low frustration tolerance” (Laufer et al. 1957, p. 38). These symptoms were placed in “associated symptoms” in DSM-III:

Associated features vary as a function of age and include obstinacy, stubbornness, negativism, bossiness, bullying, increased mood lability, low frustration tolerance, temper outbursts, low self-esteem, and lack of response to discipline. (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1980, p. 42)

The emotional symptoms seen in hyperactive children were also described in “oppositional disorder,” which was defined as a “pattern of disobedient, negativistic and provocative opposition to authority” but which included “temper tantrums” as one of the criteria (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1980). The high rate (up to 65%; e.g., Petty et al. 2009) of comorbidity between ADHD and oppositional/oppositional defiant disorder may stem from the cleavage of symptoms that previously were part of the same condition.

The description of ADHD-associated features was not changed in DSM-III-R (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1987, p. 51), but oppositional disorder began to include more of what Stringaris & Goodman (2009) considered “mood” symptoms: “Often loses temper, is often touchy or easily annoyed by others, is often angry and resentful.” Unfortunately, the structured and semistructured interviews that emerged after DSM-III was published focused only on the primary criteria of psychiatric disorders like ADHD. It became impossible to test whether deleting the mood symptoms from the construct of ADHD was a valid decision. Some investigators felt that it was a wise decision because those children were thought to have a mood disorder, and the creation of a more liberal definition of mania/bipolar disorder began to surface (e.g., Biederman et al. 1995, 1996; Weinberg et al. 1997); however, others disagreed (Barkley & Fischer 2010).

Changes in the Conceptualization of Mania/Bipolar Disorder

Mania and the condition in which it occurs (initially “manic depressive illness” and then bipolar disorder) also had both a name and a concept change from DSM-II to DSM-III. DSM-II (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1968) provided little diagnostic guidance, but the spirit of the disorder’s definition prior to 1980 was kept in the clinical description in the tenth revision of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10; World Health Organ. 1992), which contrasts somewhat with the symptom-based DSM approach. That is, DSM-III and subsequent iterations operationalize specific symptoms that occur during the course of mania, emphasizing a “distinct period [lasting at least a week] during which expansive, irritable or elevated mood symptoms occur with associated symptoms.” When an individual endorses enough of these symptoms, s/he is given the diagnosis of a depressive and/or manic episode. However, three important qualifiers were not made explicit for mania, as they were for depression. That is, criteria for major depression specify that symptoms should occur most of the day every day; depressive symptoms themselves should co-occur; and, finally, symptoms should represent a change from previous functioning. This has led to different interpretations of how persistent manic symptoms should be and whether or not mania should represent a change, as well as how that change should be established. We submit that more conservative bipolar diagnosticians believed that mania should follow the same rules as depression; those who interpreted the criteria literally did not.

In addition, the course of illness was not built into the definition of bipolar disorder. This approach is somewhat different from the ICD-10 clinical version (World Health Organ. 1992), which relies on pattern recognition to define bipolar disorder. The ICD-10 text definition of bipolar disorder requires that the patient experience repeated (i.e., at least two) episodes in which mood, energy, and activity levels are significantly disturbed; that recovery is usually complete between episodes; and that manic episodes usually begin abruptly and last for between two weeks and four to five months (median duration about four months). Depressions tend to last longer (median length about six months), although rarely for more than a year, except in the elderly. The first episode may occur at any age from childhood to old age, with a variable pattern of remission and relapse. The ICD-10 definition is clearly a more narrow and conservative view of mania/BP I disorder than that of the DSM.

Subsequent alterations in DSM mania criteria also had a downstream impact. Irritability, encompassed in the concept of “dysphoric mood” in DSM-III (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1980), was eliminated from the depression criteria in DSM-III-R (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1987) and later DSMs and was only ascertained in mania (except for children and adolescents, where irritable mood still counts toward a diagnosis of depression). Simultaneously, the duration requirement of at least one week for the “distinct period” was eliminated, meaning that a distinct period could last from minutes to hours, thus confusing mood lability with manic episodes. Finally, the criterion of at least one week of a distinct period does not necessarily clarify how to define the offset of an episode. That is, if symptoms start but never end, they have lasted at least one week. Without an offset, there is no real episode. A focus on symptoms without a comprehensive longitudinal history that defines episodes makes it difficult to distinguish a hyperactive, distractible, impulsive, emotionally labile, overly reactive, and irritable child with ADHD from one with mania.

Indeed, the development of a condition called severe mood dysregulation (SMD) emerged as the result of a research effort to distinguish chronic irritability from episodic irritability (Leibenluft et al. 2003, 2011) that may suggest mania. Symptoms of SMD include both chronic irritability and frequent and intense explosive outbursts. However, also included in the definition are the “B” symptoms of mania, many of which are also symptoms of ADHD (e.g., restlessness, distractibility, intrusiveness, pressured speech). It should be no surprise, then, that 75% to 80% of children with SMD in clinical samples carry diagnoses of comorbid ADHD (from the “B” mania symptoms) and oppositional defiant disorder (often loses temper, touchy, and easily annoyed) (Brotman et al. 2006). Disruptive mood dysregulation disorder, the DSM-5 committee’s reformulation of SMD (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 2013), has put the whole issue on the clinical map for the coming years. Notably, however, although the committee eliminated the ADHD-like symptoms from the disruptive mood dysregulation disorder criteria, rates of ADHD are still high (Axelson et al. 2012, Margulies et al. 2012). There are many important exclusionary criteria for disruptive mood dysregulation disorder, too, and it remains to be seen whether it will provide a useful alternative for erroneously diagnosed bipolar disorder or cause clinicians to miss the diagnosis of ADHD.

We submit, then, that it is not the symptoms that are so different between mania and ADHD but rather the illness course. ADHD is a developmental disorder beginning in early childhood that, although fluctuating, is chronic. Mania occurs in episodes defined as clear differences from one’s previous behavior and function (Carlson 1990). Changes from DSM-II (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1968) to DSM-III (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1980) that de-emphasized emotionality unintentionally caused the relabeling of some children who previously would have been considered as having hyperkinesis/ADHD, placing them in a bipolar spectrum where episodicity was inadvertently neglected.

Structured and Semistructured Interviews Collect Different Information

Another issue relevant to understanding the confusion that has developed over identifying children with mania is that the criteria that were established emerged from a very long history of understanding the condition in adults. There was no guidance on how to assess the condition in children and adolescents and thus no wrestling with developmental or assessment considerations (Carlson & Meyer 2006). The DSM adds developmental qualifiers for most conditions that begin in childhood (e.g., the symptom of inattention in ADHD is described as maladaptive and inconsistent with developmental level). However, there are no developmental qualifiers for mania or mood regulation, probably because we don’t know what the developmental qualifiers are rather than because they are unnecessary. Nevertheless, if investigators don’t know that they need to collect such information, we are going to be (and are) no wiser in the future.

Child and adolescent psychiatric assessment has wrestled with informant issues for years (De los Reyes & Kazdin 2005; Youngstrom et al. 2003, 2006). The general consensus has been to believe adult informants (parents and teachers) for behaviors that are observable (e.g., hyperactivity, inattention, impulsivity, and aggression) and to believe the child (or person experiencing the internal symptom) for symptoms such as depression or anxiety. Even if that heuristic is entirely valid, mania presents a particularly complicated set of issues because some of the symptoms are observable, and some are more internal (Carlson & Meyer 2006).

Systematic assessments of symptoms have provided a way to increase the reliability of diagnoses (Endicott & Spitzer 1978). By asking questions in the same or similar ways, the hope has been to eliminate the variability that occurs when interviewers decide arbitrarily what to ask or how to ask it. However, structured and semistructured interviews make no attempt to collect a history systematically (Andreasen 2007), and interviews are inconsistent in how they operationalize the DSM criteria and manic symptoms (Carlson 2011, Galanter et al. 2012).

At least six child and adolescent interviews have been designed to assess DSM-IV (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1994) diagnoses in general and mania/bipolar disorder in particular (Galanter et al. 2012). Where there are parent and child informants, the question of whose information should be counted is addressed by one of several different algorithms (Youngstrom et al. 2006). However, the choice of algorithm has significant implications for diagnosis. Differences exist in whether both informants must agree on the presence of a symptom versus only one informant and whether the total symptom count can be contributed to by two informants. One way to get around making largely arbitrary choices between algorithms is to end the interview by bringing both informants together to reconcile differences. However, this is a luxury that often is not possible in large studies. Moreover, the order of interviewing informants is also important. The K-SADS (Kaufman et al. 1997) strongly recommends interviewing the teen before interviewing the parent. However, respecting the youth’s confidentiality prevents reconciling disparate information from the parent. Interviewing the youth after the parent allows the possibility of asking the child/teen what s/he felt like on the occasion that the parent reported the youth had experienced manic symptoms. Similarly, if a youth describes a manic episode where s/he was up all night, wore provocative clothes, and started lots of projects and a parent never noticed, the interviewer can tell the teen that the parent didn’t mention the episode and ask why it went unnoticed.

Some interviews don’t use child information at all. This addresses the problem of reconciling discrepancies by fiat. However, it does not address the concern that some experiences are likely to be described only by the child (e.g., hallucinations, racing thoughts). Teacher information is almost never solicited in studies of children with bipolar disorder, and where the issue has been addressed, teacher information has been discounted because it is felt that teachers are not qualified to identify mania (Youngstrom et al. 2003). On the other hand, teachers are qualified to recognize ADHD, so in situations where parents describe both manic symptoms and ADHD symptoms, or simply manic symptoms with hyperactivity, distractibility, and rapid speech, and teachers observe no behavior problems, questions clearly exist about whether the child could be actually experiencing manic episodes (Carlson & Blader 2011).

Most germane to the liberal-versus-conservative evolution of mania/bipolar disorder is how episodes are operationalized and how descriptions of episodes are elicited. Galanter and colleagues (2012) have detailed the degree to which interviews, which many assume are virtually identical to each other, are quite different. Differences include how episodes are ascertained in interviews, whether behaviors must be observed by others or represent a change from previous behavior, and whether the various manic symptoms co-occur. The WASH-U-KSADS (Geller et al. 2002), which elicits information on a current episode, tries to get the parent and child to date the onset and offset of each symptom, including ADHD symptoms (symptoms that count toward both ADHD and mania). The point at which the dates overlap constitutes an episode. Strictly speaking, though, during an episode, symptoms should start and stop at around the same time and not be defined cross-sectionally by the point at which they overlap.

The K-SADS has numerous versions (Ambrosini 2000), some of which are better than others in delineating episodes. In general, the mood question asks, “Has there ever been a time when you felt XXX? How long did that last?” On the one hand, subsequent symptom screenings do not specify whether symptoms occurred when the elated, expansive, or irritable mood occurred; on the other hand, the mania symptoms in the subsequent supplement do specify whether they occurred at the same time as the mood symptoms. In some, but not all, versions of the K-SADS, episodes and time frames are supposed to be recorded to provide the interviewer with a chronology of the behaviors to be discussed. The chronology is probably necessary to distinguish ADHD with oppositional defiant disorder from the onset of a manic episode. In the 1996 version of the K-SADS-Present and Lifetime (PL) version, the current episode (if present) and the most severe past episode are inquired about, although the age at the time of the worst episode is never actually solicited.

Researchers determine who to interview, and they elicit comorbidity in different ways. For example, investigators from Massachusetts General Hospital (e.g., Biederman et al. 2000) use the K-SADS-Epidemiological (E) 1994 version (Orvaschel 1994) and do not include children under 12 in the interview due to concerns that the information provided is not reliable. Investigators double-count symptoms across all disorders. Those who double-count symptoms believe that it is simply not possible to know which condition is accounting for the psychopathology and thus count the item toward both disorders. Not surprisingly, rates of comorbidity are much higher in these investigations. It is our contention that the result yields a broader conceptualization of mania and a sample that is different from investigations that take a more conservative approach and require manic symptoms to be over and above ADHD symptoms. Finally, clinicians are not used as interviewers, although the investigators provide many checks and balances to overcome this (Wozniak et al. 2003).

Galanter and colleagues (2012) imply, rather than state explicitly, that there is every likelihood that the heterogeneity within samples and across samples, because of different interviews and criteria interpretations, is quite large. The issue of heterogeneity is ironic because structured and semistructured interviews were designed to mitigate just this problem.

In summary, it is not unusual for method variance to create significant heterogeneity in diagnostic results (Youngstrom et al. 2006). For instance, in a large meta-analysis that explored methodological variables in ADHD, variables such as using a single informant versus summing criteria from either informant where there was more than one, and using DSM-IV (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1994) rather than earlier DSM criteria, increased rates of ADHD (Polanczyk et al. 2007). However, reviews and meta-analyses have virtually ignored these issues in studies of bipolar disorder in children and adolescents (Faraone et al. 2012, Kowatch et al. 2005). The following section aims to explicate the bipolar controversy and reconcile disparities in findings of bipolar disorder in youth by highlighting some of the conservative and liberal biases. Admittedly, however, reconciliation is not always possible because unidentified heterogeneity is occurring within as well as across samples.

How Methodological Issues Impact Epidemiologic, Comorbidity, High-Risk, and Longitudinal Studies

Epidemiologic Studies

Rates of bipolar disorder in community samples are influenced by all of the aforementioned methodological caveats with some added ones. The time frame studied (lifetime, past year, past three months), whether parent and/or child (usually teen or older child) is interviewed and whether mania and/or hypomania is addressed, yields different rates. An important question has been whether rates in youths are the same as in adults. On the one hand, because bipolar disorder is an early-onset condition, rates should be similar. On the other hand, because first onsets continue to accumulate through adulthood, lifetime rates should increase with age.

To simplify the discussion, we focus on rates of mania/BP I. A comprehensive meta-analysis of rates of bipolar disorder in youths was recently published by Van Meter et al. (2011), who questioned whether rates of early-onset BP I disorder are disproportionately higher in the United States than in Europe. The authors conclude that rates are similar. They also conclude that rates of mania are about 1.2%, similar to rates in at least some studies of BP I in adults (Kessler et al. 1994; Merikangas et al. 2007, 2011).

A closer look at the articles in the meta-analysis, however, reveals that very few studies provide satisfying information, and those that do have found lower rates. The study by Kim-Cohen et al. (2003), done on the New Zealand Dunedin cohort, was included in error; it did not measure mania in youth under age 18. In the Netherlands study (Verhulst et al. 1997), the ∼2% rate was achieved by adding the parent rate (1.1%) and the teen rate (0.9%). As the authors noted, there was very little overlap in symptom reports, and the authors themselves were quite skeptical of their results. Stringaris and colleagues (2010) required convergence between parent and child reports to diagnose bipolar disorder because they found a very low kappa (0.02%) between parent and child. Doing so yielded a rate of mania of 0.1%. In a much smaller community study using parent information to corroborate child data, Carlson & Kashani (1988) also found low rates (0.7%). Although the Methods for the Epidemiology of Child and Adolescent Mental Disorders study (Goodman et al. 1998) was cited as having a rate of mania of 1.3%, symptoms from either parent or child were used. Reliability for mania was never examined because the condition was too rare. Rates were likely much lower than 1.3%—closer to 0.4% to 0.6% when convergence reports were required (P. Fisher, personal communication). Nevertheless, rates are variable even when using a single informant. Using only teen reports, the lifetime rate for mania was found to be 0.1% in the Oregon Adolescent Depression Project (Lewinsohn et al. 1995) and 1.7% in the National Comorbidity Survey-Adolescents (Merikangas et al. 2012). In summary, studies designed from a liberal perspective have generally produced rates of BP I that differ from the more conservative studies. Liberal studies, where there is an absence of confirming symptoms from another informant, found rates in youths to be similar to those of adults in most instances. Studies using more narrowly defined mania and convergent informants reported lower rates.

Even when teens report symptoms that meet mania criteria, there are questions about the validity of their reports. Cicero and colleagues (2009) have raised concerns about the significance of self-reported manic symptoms in young adults. They used publicly available data from the National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions, conducted during 2001 and 2002, and a follow-up sample collected three years later (Natl. Inst. Alcohol Abuse Alcohol. 2001–2005). The sample included 43,093 noninstitutionalized, civilian respondents age 18 and over; 80% of the original sample completed the follow-up survey. The investigators found that there was a significant drop over the three-year time frame in the respondents reporting lifetime mania. Kessler et al. (2009) used the same algorithm as the National Comorbidity Survey-Adolescents and found an overall rate of 1.15% for BP I. There was a very significant age gradient by age group, however, with highest rates being in those younger than age 25. In addition, between 50% and 75% of the sample no longer endorsed manic symptoms three years later (although there does not appear to be an age gradient in that observation). The authors concluded that there is a “developmentally limited” form of bipolar disorder (i.e., young people report a manic episode during young adulthood but never have another episode, hence the decline in rates). That conclusion certainly contradicts data from clinical samples suggesting that early-onset bipolar patients are more likely to be chronic or to have multiple episodes (Birmaher et al. 2009a, Carlson et al. 2012, Geller et al. 2008, Perlis et al. 2004). The alternative conclusion is that young people from epidemiologic studies who endorse manic symptoms (along with other comorbidities) may have mental health problems, but they do not correspond to the problems of manic patients seen in clinical practice. If that is true, then studies reporting high rates of bipolar disorder in youths need to be understood in that context.

Comorbidity with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Community studies of ADHD examining rates of bipolar disorder.

The relationship between ADHD and bipolar disorder is at the heart of the debate about bipolar disorder phenomenology in youths (for a review, see Pataki & Carlson 2013). How often are the two conditions comorbid? Are they confused because of methodological problems (Carlson 1998, Kent & Craddock 2003)? Is ADHD a risk factor for subsequent bipolar disorder in adults (Carlson & Weintraub 1993), or are the emotional symptoms simply part of ADHD (Barkley & Fischer 2010, Sobansky et al. 2010)? Although well-sampled community studies should be the gold standard for identifying conditions without the bias of treatment referral, and there have been many community studies of ADHD in children (for a review, see Döpfner et al. 2008), we could find only one published report where comorbidity of ADHD with bipolar disorder was examined. In a Finnish study of ADHD in teens (Smalley et al. 2007), in which 6,622 parents returned an ADHD screening form and a subsample was evaluated with the K-SADS-PL, 6.7% of teens were identified with ADHD, 1% with bipolar disorder, and none with both. However, these were conservative diagnoses insofar as parent and teen data were examined together and discrepancies were resolved.

Community studies of bipolar disorder examining rates of ADHD.

Several community studies have examined rates of ADHD in teens with mania or manic symptoms. In a study of 150 Missouri teens using the DSM-III criteria and the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents (Herjanic & Reich 1982), 20 teens (13.3%) said they had impairing manic symptoms (symptoms of mania but not in episodes nor corroborated by parent; only 1 teen had parent-confirmed BP I) (Carlson & Kashani 1988). Of these 20, 45% had ADHD, a rate significantly higher than in symptomatic teens without manic symptoms. In the Oregon Adolescent Depression Study (Lewinsohn et al. 1995), which used the DSM-III-R criteria (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1987) and the K-SADS-E, 18 teens (0.9%) had lifetime BP I, BP II, or cyclothymia. Only 11.1% of the 18 teens had ADHD—a rate that was perhaps low because only the teens were interviewed (Barkley et al. 2002); however, this rate was much higher than in the rest of the sample (2.7%). Rates of all disruptive behavior disorders were 22.2%.

Most recently, the National Comorbidity Survey-Adolescents used the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (Kessler et al. 2009) and DSM-IV criteria (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1994) with diagnostic information from teens only (Merikangas et al. 2012). Rates of ADHD in youths with bipolar disorder varied from 18% to 24%, depending on whether the sample included both unipolar mania and mania plus depression, or only the latter. With the exception of the small Carlson & Kashani (1988) study, the above studies lacked corroborating information from another informant; only the affected teen was interviewed about ADHD symptoms. A parent informant increases the rates of ADHD diagnosis over information from the subject alone (Barkley et al. 2002) and decreases the rates of mania diagnosis (Carlson & Kashani 1988, Stringaris et al. 2010). Methodology matters.

Clinical samples of ADHD examining rates of bipolar disorder.

Rates of co-occurring bipolar disorder in children with ADHD vary widely, again depending on the assessment approach and specific procedures used by the research group (Arnold et al. 2011, Carlson 2011, Galanter et al. 2012, Galanter & Leibenluft 2008, Kent & Craddock 2003) and the age of the sample. Recent work includes a study from the United Kingdom on an ADHD sample (youths ages 6 to 18) collected for genetic research and using a three-month prevalence with the Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment (Angold et al. 1995). The study found no child with mania and only one who met DSM-IV criteria for hypomania (0.5% of the sample) (Hassan et al. 2011). Only 1.8% of youths in the Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD, ages 15 to 18 at the eight-year follow up, were reported to have even hypomanic symptoms (Molina et al. 2009) using the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (Shaffer et al. 2000). A study from Italy using the K-SADS-PL (Kaufman et al. 1997) in 129 children with ADHD found a rate of 16.7% (Masi et al. 2006), though most cases had bipolar disorder-not otherwise specified (BP-NOS), and only 1.7% had BP I. In contrast, studies taking the position that judgments cannot be made about whether shared ADHD, or other, symptoms and manic symptoms belong to one or the other condition (e.g., Biederman et al. 1996, 2004; Tillman & Geller. 2006; Wozniak et al. 1995) report rates between 11% and 23% of BP I/mania using the K-SADS-E (Orvaschel 1994) or WASH-U-KSADS (Geller et al. 2001). Perhaps not surprisingly, then, if symptoms count toward two conditions, comorbidity rates will be higher.

Perhaps the most compelling finding against the conclusion that people with ADHD develop bipolar disorder comes from long-term studies of children with ADHD, in which rates of bipolar disorder in adulthood are no higher than rates in the general population (Carlson et al. 2000, Fischer et al. 2002, Klein et al. 2012, Weiss et al. 1985). However, those studies diagnose bipolar disorder conservatively.

In summary, if the base rate of BP I disorder in teens is <1%, then conservatively defined BP I in youths with ADHD is not much higher. Rates are high when ADHD and manic symptoms are double-counted and episodes are de-emphasized.

Clinical studies of bipolar disorder examining rates of ADHD.

In clinical samples of children diagnosed with bipolar disorder, rates of ADHD also vary considerably and for the usual methodological reasons. For instance, Kowatch and colleagues (2005) combined data from seven very different studies of bipolar disorder (not necessarily BP I) and reported rates of comorbid ADHD ranging from about 10% to over 80%. Methodological differences (age of sample, quality of study quality, and number of informants) increased rates of comorbid ADHD. Not surprisingly, rates of ADHD are higher in bipolar samples where symptoms of ADHD and mania are double-counted and no judgment is made regarding whether the ADHD symptoms worsened during the episode. Thus, studies of children (versus teens) using the WASH-U-KSADS (Geller et al. 2001) typically find high rates of ADHD, ranging from 70% to 95% [e.g., 95% in the baseline episode (Geller et al. 2008), 92.8% in the Treatment of Early Age Mania study (Geller et al. 2012), approximately 70% in the divalproex-extended release study (Wagner et al. 2009), and 75% in the oxcarbazepine study (Wagner et al. 2006)]. Similarly, studies using DSM-III-R criteria (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1987) and the 1994 version of the K-SADS-E (e.g., Biederman et al. 1996, 2004; Faraone et al. 1997; West et al. 1995; Wozniak et al. 1995) find rates as high as 87%. Studies using the K-SADS- PL (Kaufman et al. 1997) find slightly lower rates of ADHD in bipolar samples, i.e., 60% to 70% (Axelson et al. 2006, Brotman et al. 2006). To the degree that investigators believe in the importance of clear episodes (see http://www.aacap.org/cs/BipolarDisorder.ResourceCenter) and train their interviewers accordingly, rates of ADHD appear to be somewhat lower.

Informant corroboration on the presence of symptoms in more than one setting is important for the diagnosis of ADHD. Parents and teachers are the usual informants because they represent different and clinically relevant settings. Optimally, investigators obtain information from both rather than rely on one source to speculate on what is happening elsewhere. Requiring documentation of symptoms from both parents and teachers substantially lowers rates found for ADHD. For mania, the change in the person’s behavior and function that identifies the actual episode should be confirmed by an informant other than the subject, who may experience symptoms but lack insight on their impact. As noted previously, however, not all interviews ask whether a change has been noticed by others or clarify whether the behavior change is clearly due to mania. Not surprisingly, when information is obtained from a second source, rates found for mania are lower. For instance, Carlson & Blader (2011) examined children whose parents endorsed severe manic symptoms but who were not identified as manic or even hyperactive by teacher ratings. In those instances, the children were actually found by best-estimate diagnosis to have depressive or anxiety disorders and not mania (Carlson & Blader 2011).

The effect of other informants may also be seen in the multisite Longitudinal Assessment of Manic Symptoms study (Arnold et al. 2011). Parents of 707 outpatients rated their child’s level of manic symptoms on the General Behavior Inventory (Youngstrom et al. 2008). Parents and children were subsequently interviewed with the K-SADS-PL (Kaufman et al. 1997). LAMS investigators found that 69% of children with BP I had comorbid ADHD and, on the basis of parent reports, groups of children were identified as having BP I, ADHD, or both. The BP + ADHD children had the most severe parent symptom ratings. Teacher information on the same children, however, indicated that the children with BP + ADHD by parent report did not differ on manic symptoms from those with ADHD without BP (i.e., symptoms were less severe than parents reported), and most importantly, children whose parents endorsed BP only with severe symptoms did not differ by teacher report from those without any diagnosis. In other words, many children with parent-reported severe manic symptoms do not have those symptoms in school and therefore didn’t have them most of the day every day. Conservatively speaking then, they would not get a BP I diagnosis.

A developmental relationship between ADHD and bipolar disorder likely accounts for the varying rates with which they seem to co-occur at different ages. As originally noted in a study by Faraone and colleagues (1997), the reported rate of comorbid ADHD in youths with bipolar disorder was 93% in children, 88% in teens with a childhood onset, and 59% in teens with adolescent-onset mania. The Course and Outcome of Bipolar Youth study similarly reported rates of ADHD in their bipolar subjects (including all of the bipolar spectrum, not just BP I) that varied by age as follows: 71.7% in children under age 12, 69.3% in teens with childhood-onset bipolar disorder, and 31.1% in adolescents with adolescent-onset bipolar disorder (Birmaher et al. 2009c). In adults with bipolar disorder, rates of ADHD/childhood externalizing disorders are lower still and have been generally reported to be between 10% and 20% (Bernardi et al. 2010, Carlson et al. 2002, Perlis et al. 2004, Sachs et al. 2000).

In summary, rates of bipolar disorder in children with ADHD depend on how mania has been elicited and defined, the comparison group, and the age of the sample studied. If multiple informants are required and mania is described as a clear episode superimposed on ADHD, as one sees in teens and adults, the rates are much lower. On the other hand, as noted in the community studies, data are consistent with finding elevated rates of ADHD in youths with mania/bipolar disorder. Some hypothesize that this represents a specific subtype (Arnold et al. 2011, Biederman et al. 2000, Carlson 1998).

Liberal and Conservative Views of Top-Down High-Risk Studies

Rates of bipolar disorder are significantly higher in the offspring of bipolar parents than in the offspring of normal controls. In contrast, studies differ on whether ADHD is more common in the offspring of bipolar parents than controls. For both disorders, rates vary widely, again depending on the age of the sample; whether mania or the bipolar spectrum is measured; whether the concept of mania/bipolar disorder is construed broadly or narrowly, and for this issue, the origin of the proband sample; and the nature of the comparison group (e.g., representative or “supernormal”; Duffy et al. 2011).

A meta-analysis of studies of bipolar offspring completed before 1995 (LaPalme et al. 1997) found rates of BP I disorder to be 5.4% compared to 0% in the offspring of parents without bipolar disorder. Since then, a number of studies have been done, all but one of which used a prospective follow-up design, and the findings are much more variable (Carlson 2013). Conservatively diagnosed assessment approaches reported rates of 2% to 6% for BP I disorder (Duffy et al. 2007, Mesman et al. 2013, Meyer et al. 2004, Nurnberger et al. 2011, Shaw et al. 2005). These rates contrast with another group of high-risk studies using either the WASH-U-KSADS or the KSADS-E in which rates of bipolar I disorder were 10% to 33% in offspring (Chang et al. 2000, Henin et al. 2005, Hirshfeld-Becker et al. 2006, Singh et al. 2007).

Similar differences are found in rates of ADHD, which were between 0% and 8% in conservatively diagnosed samples (i.e., similar to population rates; see review by Duffy 2012). With two exceptions (Birmaher et al. 2009b, 2010), studies finding high rates of bipolar disorder in the offspring of bipolar parents also found rates of ADHD that were high (15% to 30%) and significantly greater than rates of ADHD in comparison groups (Chang et al. 2000, Henin et al. 2005, Hirshfeld-Becker et al. 2006, Singh et al. 2007).

Duffy and co-investigators (2011, 2012) reviewed the high-risk literature to explain the range of rates of bipolar disorder and ADHD in various studies. They found that the source of the parent probands made a significant difference. Where parents were identified through neurobiologic research, with parent and child diagnosis obtained through best-estimate procedures, rates of parent comorbidity were lower, rates of psychopathology in the nonproband parent were lower, and rates of BP I were lower in the offspring, who themselves had an adolescent or older age of onset and low rates of ADHD. Self-referred bipolar probands recruited from advertisements or recruited from tertiary care clinics, who were diagnosed with structured interviews and whose children were assessed only with structured interviews, were found to have more offspring with psychopathology, including higher rates of ADHD and a younger age of onset of mental health problems.

The meta-analysis by Faraone and colleagues (2012) of 27 family genetic studies examining the comorbidity between ADHD and BP I disorder illustrates how findings are shaped by interpretation, study design, and selection of studies. The meta-analysis concluded that the relative risk of ADHD in bipolar probands is 2.2 (95% CI 2.1–3.2), with rates of ADHD in offspring of bipolar versus comparison probands of 27% versus 9.6%, in siblings 30.1% versus 11.0%, and in parents 16.5% versus 4.5%. Conversely, the relative risk of bipolar disorder in ADHD probands is 1.8 (95% CI 1.3–2.6), with rates of bipolar disorder in offspring of ADHD probands versus comparison probands being 6.8% versus 3.5%, in siblings 5.9% versus 2.8%, and in parents 5.1% versus 3.1%.

Pertinent to this discussion, the highest relative risks of ADHD in offspring and other relatives of probands with bipolar disorder were from studies using the WASH-U-KSADS and the K-SADS-E. Closer inspection revealed that in two of the studies (i.e., Carlson & Weintraub 1993, Duffy et al. 2007), offspring had learning problems or ADHD symptoms but not an ADHD diagnosis. Additionally, the study by Zahn-Waxler et al. (1988) stated that two of seven offspring of bipolar parents had ADHD. Not stated was that this conclusion was drawn from a parent rating scale. Follow-up interviews of the parent and child yielded no children with ADHD. Neither of the Dutch bipolar high-risk studies (e.g., Hillegers et al. 2005, Wals et al. 2001) nor the Amish study (Shaw et al. 2005), all of which reported low rates of ADHD, were included in the meta-analysis. All told, it would appear that rates of ADHD in bipolar offspring were about the same as population rates (around 10% when all studies are considered). What made the odds ratio high were the low rates in comparison groups that were usually selected to have no psychopathology (“supernormals”).

Finally, in Faraone’s (2012) meta-analysis, just as studies of ADHD among relatives of bipolar disorder found the highest relative risks using the WASH-U-KSADS or K-SADS-E, so did studies of bipolar disorder among relatives of ADHD probands. The one exception came from a study where 2 of 112 (1.78%) fathers with ADHD were said to have mania, basically the same as the population rate (Bhatia et al. 1991). No mention was made of rates of ADHD in the comparison group, which precludes an accurate risk ratio.

In summary, rates of BP I in offspring of BP I probands are almost always higher than in a nonbipolar comparison group. How high they are depends on whether mania is defined conservatively or liberally and on how complicated and strictly diagnosed the bipolar disorder in the parent probands are. These conclusions apply equally to the relationship of ADHD and bipolar disorder, whether the focus is on rates of BP I disorder in samples with ADHD or on rates of ADHD in samples of BP I disorder.

Longitudinal Course of Bipolar Disorder

The most pressing question about childhood-onset bipolar disorder (and especially mania/BP I disorder) is whether it is continuous with bipolar disorder in adulthood. Manic symptoms, whether or not they occur in episodes, are clearly impairing in children. The question is one of homotypic versus heterotypic continuity: Is mania the same disorder starting younger, does it evolve into something else, was it something else all along, or, in some cases, does it remit completely? These issues are further complicated by the fact that BP I disorder in adults is heterogeneous in its phenomenology and outcome, and although there are many short-term outcome studies, very few are longer than four years. In child- and adolescent-onset studies, a minimum of a four-year follow-up is necessary for the samples to enter adolescence and in some cases young adulthood.

A number of follow-up studies of adults with BP I disorder have examined overall outcome. Although when the study took place, the sample composition and definitions of outcome differ and some studies have sample overlap, it is possible to combine information into (a) recovered, remitted, good outcome; (b) incomplete remission or fair outcome; and (c) chronic or poor outcome. With this metric, a little less than half of the samples do fairly well (they experience no recurrences, or if there is a recurrence, good functioning resumes when the episodes remit), approximately one-quarter of the samples do poorly (they are chronically ill in one or another mood state, they attempt or commit suicide, or they become chronically psychotic), and outcomes for the remaining one-quarter of the samples are in between (Angst & Sellaro 2000; Angst et al. 2003; Carlson et al. 1974, 2012; Coryell et al. 1998; Goldberg & Harrow 2011; Morrison et al. 1973; Stephens & McHugh 1991; Tsuang & Dempsey 1979).

Only three of the studies cited above have followed participants for four or more years and provide the kind of sample and follow-up detail that allows some comparison with child and adolescent follow-up studies: Angst’s Zurich sample, the Collaborative Depression Study, and the Suffolk County Mental Heath Project (Angst et al. 2003, Solomon et al. 2010, and Carlson et al. 2012, respectively). In these studies, where follow-up duration ranges from 4 to more than 30 years, 40% to 50% of adult participants were male, 89% to 100% had been initially hospitalized, and 55% to 100% were psychotic at their index episode. Their age at the time of study entry was late 20s to early 30s; age of onset was approximately 25 years. As noted above, overall outcome is decent for almost half the sample, and the rate of chronicity and poor outcome is approximately 20% to 25%. Comorbidity was not a focus of these studies, although in two studies that used data from other papers on the same samples, ADHD/externalizing disorders occurred in a little over 20% of the adult samples (Carlson et al. 2002, Winokur et al. 1993). Median manic episode duration was 7 to 16 weeks, and median time ill over follow-up was about 20%.

The impact of childhood psychopathology—either prior externalizing disorders (ADHD and/or conduct disorders) or other childhood psychopathology (anxiety disorders, depressive symptoms, adjustment disorders)—on outcome in bipolar disorder has been examined in the Suffolk County Mental Health Project (Carlson et al. 2002, 2012). The study reported that childhood psychopathology prior to the onset of BP I contributed substantially to the global assessment of functioning at both the 24-month and 48-month follow-ups and was more predictive of functioning and length of time ill than was age of onset. In terms of influence on episode duration and recurrence, among the significant predictors of time to remission were young age of onset and childhood psychopathology. Almost two-thirds of remitters relapsed by four-year follow-up, mostly to a manic episode. The predictors of time to relapse were depressive symptoms during childhood (Bromet et al. 2005).

Adolescent-onset bipolar studies have smaller samples and shorter-term follow-up (four to five years) (Jairam et al. 2004, Srinath et al. 1998, Strober et al. 1995). As with adult studies, half the patients in these studies were male and were hospitalized for their index episode, which was usually manic or mixed. They were ages 14 to 16 and had an adolescent onset. Rates of psychosis varied from 28% to 63%. Rates of externalizing comorbidity were low (4% to 15%). The index manic episode duration was 8 to 16 weeks. Recovery rates were almost 100% if follow-up was long enough, but relapse over follow-up ranged from 44% to 67%—higher if subthreshold symptoms are counted (Strober et al. 1995). In the two samples from India, about half the teens had a good recovery, about 20% were chronically ill, and the remainder of the teens were somewhat impaired. These adolescent samples, then, appeared to be roughly similar to their adult counterparts in terms of their relatively classic description and outcome.

There is one follow-up study of children (Wozniak et al. 2011) and two follow-up studies of children and adolescents (Birmaher et al. 2009a, Geller et al. 2008) that are four years or longer. In studies by Wozniak and co-investigators (2011) and Geller and colleagues (2008), the samples were diagnosed with current mania/BP I at the time of study entry. In the Course of Bipolar Youth study (Axelson et al. 2006, Birmaher et al. 2009a), it is unclear how many subjects were symptomatic with mania at study entry because worst episode rather than current status was reported. Age comparisons included the entire bipolar spectrum (BP I, BP II, and BP-NOS), but in the four-year follow-up data, BP I subjects were described separately from the BP II and BP-NOS subjects.

Compared to the adolescent-onset and adult studies reported above, children in the Massachusetts General Hospital (Wozniak et al. 2011) and Washington University (Geller et al. 2008) bipolar outcome studies were all outpatients, predominantly male, and had very high rates of ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder (>80%). These studies did not delineate episodes and counted the same symptoms toward ADHD and mania. Most of the children did not recover or had very long first episodes of mania (mean 142.7 ± 139.4 weeks). Most of the Massachusetts General Hospital sample (93.6%), who were still children and young adolescents at the four-year follow-up, either continued to meet full diagnostic criteria for BP I disorder (73.1%), or continued to have persistent subthreshold BP I disorder (6.4%). In the Washington University study, where more than half the sample was over age 18 at follow-up, respondents spent 60% of the follow-up period with mood disorder symptoms. As is usual with data on percent time ill, it isn’t clear how this was distributed across the sample (e.g., whether most youths were ill for 60% of the time or if some were continuously well and others ill constantly). By the age of 18, 44% had either been continuously ill or had recurrence of the same symptoms. Although tables in the manuscript (Geller et al. 2008, p. 1130) describe the sample in their first, second, and third episodes, no data described whether the children remitted completely from their mania, remained somewhat ill, or were depressed between episodes. No functional follow-up data were described (Geller et al. 2008).

In liberally diagnosed samples, follow-up studies of children with ADHD reveal comparatively high rates of development of bipolar disorder. For instance, the Washington University mania sample had an ADHD comparison group: In a six-year follow-up of these children, 22.2% developed BP I or BP II (morbidity risk of 28.5%). Interestingly, the use of stimulants was found to be a protective factor (Tillman & Geller 2006). In the Biederman et al. (2006) 10-year follow-up study of children with ADHD, 29% developed bipolar disorder compared to 3% of controls. By contrast, in other long-term follow-up studies of ADHD (Barkley et al. 2002, Carlson et al. 2000, Klein et al. 2012, Weiss et al. 1985), conservatively diagnosed bipolar disorder occurred at population rates. One interpretation of the findings in the Washington University and Massachusetts General samples is that untreated ADHD is, in fact, worsening, and some children are developing higher rates of aggressive, irritable, dysregulated behavior rather than mania.

In the Course and Outcome of Bipolar Youth study (Birmaher et al. 2009c), which used a more conservative definition of mania, the childhood-onset BP-spectrum group had more males and higher rates of ADHD than did the teen-onset group, although rates of oppositional defiant disorder were much lower in the childhood-onset group, suggesting that chronic irritability was less of a problem. The duration of mania was at least one year. The youths spent 56% of the time over the course of the follow-up with either full-syndromal or subsyndromal mood symptoms. Those with the earliest onsets had more episodes, polarity changes, and comorbidity than the teen-onset samples. Even when episodes are more central to the diagnosis of mania, childhood onset, comorbidity, and chronicity appear to be inextricably intertwined.

The best way to explain the high comorbidity of ADHD and mania in some studies, then, is to suggest that it results from counting ADHD symptoms toward both mania and ADHD. Thus, the liberal studies show poor outcome for very-early-onset bipolar disorder because many of their bipolar subjects really have severe ADHD, which is chronic. Although the co-occurrence of mania and ADHD symptoms may represent a complex bipolar subtype, the co-occurrence may also simply represent severe ADHD with mood lability. Regardless, it is both stable and chronically impairing. Interestingly, ADHD researchers are finally paying attention to the separate impact of emotion dysregulation on outcome, and the impact is not favorable (Barkley & Fischer 2010, Martel 2009, Sobanski et al. 2010).

Although the conservative studies report lower rates of comorbidity and outcomes that are not uniformly poor, even some individuals conservatively diagnosed with bipolar disorder have poor outcomes. Individuals with poor outcomes are likely to have comorbid disorders (or, in adults, a childhood history of nonbipolar psychopathology).

Conclusion

The premise of this review is that conservative and liberal philosophies have influenced how investigators design and interpret their studies on child- and adolescent-onset bipolar disorder. The conservative viewpoint is that BP I disorder is defined with distinct episodes of mania that are clearly different from the person’s usual functioning and behavior. The difference is best identified either by a convergence of identified patient information with parent reports or by some other source documenting that there has been a change in functioning and behavior. Within the episode, although developmental factors and contexts must be considered, the manic symptoms are basically as they are defined in adults. Although comorbidity with ADHD may occur, unless there is significant change from baseline, ADHD symptoms should not be used toward the diagnosis of mania. Irritability also must be part of the episode of mania and not a symptom that is borrowed from other conditions and merged into the mania criteria. Defined this way, mania is uncommon prior to puberty and increases in frequency thereafter.

A more liberal viewpoint would argue that the above definition is too narrow. Any informant can identify manic symptoms, or multiple informants can each provide unique information, supporting the “OR” rule (i.e. symptoms can come from one informant OR the other without their necessarily agreeing) and leading to higher prevalence rates. In addition, given how difficult it is to determine which symptoms, in children with many problems, are due to which condition (and there is no question that this is difficult), symptoms shared by comorbid conditions should be counted toward all of them. For example, irritability can be counted toward depression, generalized anxiety, mania, oppositional defiant disorder, etc. Episodes are given less weight than the DSM-IV symptoms because episodes have, in fact, been poorly defined by the criteria and the structured interviews designed to elicit these criteria.

Although it is important to compare the relative validity of the liberal and conservative positions, the larger lesson is that the samples and results acquired with these divergent philosophies are dissimilar. Differences are apparent, not surprisingly, in prevalence, rates of co-occurring ADHD, rates of bipolar disorder and ADHD in family/genetic studies, and outcome. The way information is obtained in structured or semistructured interviews precludes being able to re-examine one’s sample and determine which child was liberally versus conservatively diagnosed, as there is no way to reconstruct the episode. Both perspectives can claim evidence for reliability and validity that supports their position. That is to say, investigators with the same philosophy agree about what they are defining, and they use the same philosophy to identify symptoms in family members and in follow-up studies. Thus, the good news may be that each philosophy is internally consistent. The bad news is that the conservative and liberal philosophies are so different that it is extremely difficult to compare studies conducted from these different perspectives.

Literature Cited

Ambrosini PJ. 2000. Historical development and present status of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children (K-SADS). J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39:49–58CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1980. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: Am. Psychiatr. Publ. 3rd ed.Google Scholar

Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1987. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: Am. Psychiatr. Publ. 3rd ed., rev.Google Scholar

Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: Am. Psychiatr. Publ. 4th ed.Google Scholar

Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 2000. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: Am. Psychiatr. Publ. 4th ed., text rev.Google Scholar

Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: Am. Psychiatr. Publ. 5th ed.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Andreasen NC. 2007. DSM and the death of phenomenology in America: an example of unintended consequences. Schizophr. Bull. 33:108–12CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Angold A, Prendergast M, Cox A, et al.. 1995. The Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment (CAPA). Psychol. Med. 25:739–53CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Angst J, Gamma A, Sollaro R, et al.. 2003. Recurrence of bipolar disorders and major depression: a life-long perspective. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 253:236–40CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Angst J, Sellaro R. 2000. Historical perspectives and natural history of bipolar disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 15(48):445–57CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Anthony EJ, Scott P. 1960. Manic-depressive psychosis in childhood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1:53–72CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Arnold LE, Demeter C, Mount K, et al.. 2011. Pediatric bipolar spectrum disorder and ADHD. Comparison and comorbidity in the LAMS clinical sample. Bipolar Disord. 13:509–21CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Axelson D, Birmaher B, Strober M, et al.. 2006. Phenomenology of children and adolescents with bipolar spectrum disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63:1139–48CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Axelson D, Findling RL, Fristad MA, et al.. 2012. Examining the proposed disruptive mood dysregulation disorder diagnosis in children in the Longitudinal Assessment of Manic Symptoms study. J Clin Psychiatry 73:1342–50CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Barkley RA, Fischer M. 2010. The unique contribution of emotional impulsiveness to impairment in major life activities in hyperactive children as adults. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 49:503–13CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Barkley RA, Fischer M, Smallish L, et al.. 2002. The persistence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into young adulthood as a function of reporting source and definition of disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 111:2792–89CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Barton-Hall M. 1952. Our present knowledge about manic-depressive states in childhood. Nerv. Child. 9:319–25Google Scholar

Bernardi S, Cortese S, Solanto M, et al.. 2010. Bipolar disorder and comorbid attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. A distinct clinical phenotype? Clinical characteristics and temperamental traits. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 11:656–66CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Bhatia MS, Nigam VR, Bohra N, et al.. 1991. Attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity among paediatric outpatients. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 32:297–306CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Biederman J, Faraone S, Mick E, et al.. 1996. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and juvenile mania: an overlooked comorbidity? J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 35:997–1008CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Biederman J, Faraone SV, Wozniak J, et al.. 2004. Further evidence of unique developmental phenotypic correlates of pediatric bipolar disorder: findings from a large sample of clinically referred preadolescent children assessed over the last 7 years. J. Affect. Disord. 82(Suppl. 1):S45–58CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Biederman J, Klein RG, Pine DS, et al.. 1998. Resolved: Mania is mistaken for ADHD in prepubertal children. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 37:1091–96; discussion 1096–99CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Biederman J, Mick E, Faraone SV, et al.. 2000. Pediatric mania: a developmental subtype of bipolar disorder? Biol. Psychiatry 48(6):458–66CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Mick E. 2006. Young adult outcome of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a controlled 10-year follow-up study. Psychol. Med. 2:167–79CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Biederman J, Wozniak J, Kiely K, et al.. 1995. CBCL clinical scales discriminate prepubertal children with structured interview-derived diagnosis of mania from those with ADHD. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 34:464–71CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Birmaher B, Axelson D, Goldstein B, et al.. 2009a. Four-year longitudinal course of children and adolescents with bipolar spectrum disorders: the Course and Outcome of Bipolar Youth (COBY) study. Am. J. Psychiatry 166:795–804CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Birmaher B, Axelson D, Goldstein B, et al.. 2010. Psychiatric disorders in preschool offspring of parents with bipolar disorder: the Pittsburgh Bipolar Offspring Study (BIOS). Am. J. Psychiatry 167:321–30CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Birmaher B, Axelson D, Monk K, et al.. 2009b. Lifetime psychiatric disorders in school-aged offspring of parents with bipolar disorder: the Pittsburgh Bipolar Offspring Study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66:287–96CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Birmaher B, Axelson D, Strober M, et al.. 2009c. Comparison of manic and depressive symptoms between children and adolescents with bipolar spectrum disorders. Bipolar Disord. 11:52–62CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Blader JC, Carlson GA. 2007. Increased rates of bipolar disorder diagnoses among US child, adolescent, and adult inpatients, 1996–2004. Biol. Psychiatry 62:107–14CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Bromet EJ, Finch SJ, Carlson GA, et al.. 2005. Time to remission and relapse after the first hospital admission in severe bipolar disorder. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 40:106–13CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Brotman MA, Schmajuk M, Rich BA, et al.. 2006. Prevalence, clinical correlates, and longitudinal course of severe mood dysregulation in children. Biol. Psychiatry 60:991–97CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Carlson GA. 1990. Annotation: child and adolescent mania—diagnostic considerations. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 31:331–41CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Carlson GA. 1998. Bipolar disorder and attention deficit disorder—comorbidity or confusion. J. Affect. Disord. 51:177–89CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Carlson GA. 2011. Will the child with mania please stand up? Br. J. Psychiatry 198:171–72CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Carlson GA. 2013. Childhood ADHD does not appear to predict development of bipolar disorder among children of bipolar parents. Evid.-Based Ment. Health 16:37CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Carlson GA, Blader JC. 2011. Diagnostic implications of informant disagreement of manic symptoms. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 5:399–405CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Carlson GA, Bromet EJ, Driessens C, et al.. 2002. Age at onset, childhood psychopathology, and 2-year outcome in psychotic bipolar disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 159:307–9CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Carlson GA, Glovinsky I. 2009. The concept of bipolar disorder in children: a history of the bipolar controversy. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 18:257–71CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Carlson GA, Kashani JH. 1988. Manic symptoms in a non-referred adolescent population. J. Affect. Disord. 15:219–26CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Carlson GA, Kotin J, Davenport YB, et al.. 1974. Follow-up of 53 bipolar manic-depressive patients. Br. J. Psychiatry 124:134–39CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Carlson GA, Kotov R, Chang SW, et al.. 2012. Early determinants of four-year clinical outcomes in bipolar disorder with psychosis. Bipolar Disord. 14:19–30CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Carlson GA, Loney J, Salisbury H, et al.. 2000. Stimulant treatment in young boys with symptoms suggesting childhood mania: a report from a longitudinal study. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 10:175–84CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Carlson GA, Meyer SE. 2006. Phenomenology and diagnosis of bipolar disorder in children, adolescents, and adults: complexities and developmental issues. Dev. Psychopathol. 18:939–69CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Carlson GA, Weintraub S. 1993. Childhood behavior problems and bipolar disorder—relationship or coincidence? J. Affect. Disord. 28:143–53CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Chang KD, Steiner H, Ketter TA. 2000. Psychiatric phenomenology of child and adolescent bipolar offspring. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39:4534–60CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Cicero DC, Epler AJ, Sher KJ. 2009. Are there developmentally limited forms of bipolar disorder? J. Abnorm. Psychol. 118:431–47CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Coryell W, Turvey C, Endicott J, et al.. 1998. Bipolar I affective disorder: predictors of outcome after 15 years. J. Affect. Disord. 50:109–16CrossrefGoogle Scholar

De Los Reyes A, Kazdin AE. 2005. Informant discrepancies in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: a critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for further study. Psychol. Bull. 131:483–509CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Döpfner M, Breuer D, Wille N, et al.. 2008. How often do children meet ICD-10/DSM-IV criteria of attention deficit-/hyperactivity disorder and hyperkinetic disorder? Parent-based prevalence rates in a national sample—results of the BELLA study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 17 (Suppl. 1):59–70CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Duffy A, Alda M, Crawford L, et al.. 2007. The early manifestations of bipolar disorder: a longitudinal prospective study of the offspring of bipolar parents. Bipolar Disord. 9:828–38CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Duffy A. 2012. The nature of the association between childhood ADHD and the development of bipolar disorder: a review of prospective high-risk studies. Am. J. Psychiatry 169:1247–55CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Duffy A, Doucette S, Lewitzka U, et al.. 2011. Findings from bipolar offspring studies: methodology matters. Early Interv. Psychiatry 5:181–91CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Endicott J, Spitzer RL. 1978. A diagnostic interview: the schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 35:837–44CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Faraone SV, Biederman J, Mennin D, et al.. 1997. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with bipolar disorder: a familial subtype? J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 36:1378–87CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Faraone SV, Biederman J, Wozniak J. 2012. Examining the comorbidity between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and bipolar I disorder: a meta-analysis of family genetic studies. Am. J. Psychiatry 169:1256–66CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Feighner JP, Robins E, Guze SB, et al.. 1972. Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric research. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 26:57–63CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Fischer M, Barkley RA, Smallish L, et al.. 2002. Young adult follow-up of hyperactive children: self-reported psychiatric disorders, comorbidity, and the role of childhood conduct problems and teen CD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 30:463–75CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Galanter CA, Hundt SR, Goyal P, et al.. 2012. Variability among research diagnostic interview instruments in the application of DSM-IV-TR criteria for pediatric bipolar disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 51:605–21CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Galanter CA, Leibenluft E. 2008. Frontiers between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and bipolar disorder. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 17:325–46CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Geller B, Luby JL, Joshi P, et al.. 2012. A randomized controlled trial of risperidone, lithium, or divalproex sodium for initial treatment of bipolar I disorder, manic or mixed phase, in children and adolescents. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69:515–28CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Geller B, Tillman R, Bolhofner K, et al.. 2008. Child bipolar I disorder: prospective continuity with adult bipolar I disorder; characteristics of second and third episodes; predictors of 8-year outcome. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 65:1125–33CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Geller B, Zimerman B, Williams M, et al.. 2001. Reliability of the Washington University in St. Louis Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (WASH-U-KSADS) mania and rapid cycling sections. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40:450–55CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Geller B, Zimerman B, Williams M, et al.. 2002. Phenomenology of prepubertal and early adolescent bipolar disorder: examples of elated mood, grandiose behaviors, decreased need for sleep, racing thoughts and hypersexuality. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 12:3–9CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Goldberg JF, Harrow M. 2011. A 15-year prospective follow-up of bipolar affective disorders: comparisons with unipolar nonpsychotic depression. Bipolar Disord. 13:155–63CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Goodman SH, Hoven CW, Narrow WE, et al.. 1998. Measurement of risk for mental disorders and competence in a psychiatric epidemiologic community survey: the National Institute of Mental Health. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 33:162–73CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Harms E. 1952. Differential patterns of manic-depressive disease in childhood. Nerv. Child. 9:326–56Google Scholar

Harrington R, Myatt T. 2003. Is preadolescent mania the same condition as adult mania? A British perspective. Biol. Psychiatry 53:961–69CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Hassan A, Agha SS, Langley K, et al.. 2011. Prevalence of bipolar disorder in children and adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Br. J. Psychiatry 198:195–98CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Henin A, Biederman J, Mick E. 2005. Psychopathology in the offspring of parents with bipolar disorder: a controlled study. Biol. Psychiatry 58:554–61CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Herjanic B, Reich W. 1982. Development of a structured psychiatric interview for children: agreement between child and parent on individual symptoms. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 10:307–24CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Hillegers MH, Reichart CG, Wals M, et al.. 2005. Five-year prospective outcome of psychopathology in the adolescent offspring of bipolar parents. Bipolar Disord. 7:344–50CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Hirshfeld-Becker DR, Biederman J, Henin A. 2006. Psychopathology in the young offspring of parents with bipolar disorder: a controlled pilot study. Psychiatry Res. 145:155–67CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Jairam R, Srinath S, Girimaji SC, et al.. 2004. A prospective 4–5 year follow-up of juvenile onset bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 6:386–94CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, et al.. 1997. Schedule of Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 36:980–88CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Kent L, Craddock N. 2003. Is there a relationship between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and bipolar disorder? J. Affect. Disord. 73:211–21CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Kessler RC, Avenevoli S, Green J, et al.. 2009. National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A): III. Concordance of DSM-IV/CIDI diagnoses with clinical reassessments. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 48:386–99CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Zhao S, et al.. 1994. Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 51:8–19CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Kim-Cohen J, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, et al.. 2003. Prior juvenile diagnoses in adults with mental disorder: developmental follow-back of a prospective-longitudinal cohort. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 60:709–17CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Klein RG, Mannuzza S, Olazagasti MA, et al.. 2012. Clinical and functional outcome of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder 33 years later. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69:1295–303CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Kowatch RA, Youngstrom EA, Danielyan A, et al.. 2005. Review and meta-analysis of the phenomenology and clinical characteristics of mania in children and adolescents. Bipolar Disord. 7:483–96CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Kraepelin E. 1921. Manic Depressive Insanity and Paranoia. Edinburgh: LivingstoneCrossrefGoogle Scholar

Lapalme M, Hodgins S, LaRoche C. 1997. Children of parents with bipolar disorder: a metaanalysis of risk for mental disorders. Can. J. Psychiatry 42:623–31CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Laufer MW, Denhoff E, Solomons G. 1957. Hyperkinetic impulse disorder in children’s behavior problems. Psychosom. Med. 19:38–49CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Leibenluft E. 2011. Severe mood dysregulation, irritability, and the diagnostic boundaries of bipolar disorder in youths. Am. J. Psychiatry 168:129–42CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Leibenluft E, Charney DS, Towbin KE, et al.. 2003. Defining clinical phenotypes of juvenile mania. Am. J. Psychiatry 160:430–37CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Lewinsohn PM, Klein DN, Seeley JR. 1995. Bipolar disorders in a community sample of older adolescents: prevalence, phenomenology, comorbidity, and course. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 34:454–63CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Margulies DM, Weintraub S, Basile J, et al.. 2102. Will disruptive mood dysregulation disorder reduce false diagnosis of bipolar disorder in children? Bipolar Disord. 14(5):488–96CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Martel MM. 2009. A new perspective on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: emotion dysregulation and trait models. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50:1042–51CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Masi G, Perugi G, Toni C, et al.. 2006. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder—bipolar comorbidity in children and adolescents. Bipolar Disord. 8:373–81CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Merikangas KR, Akiskal HS, Angst J, et al.. 2007. Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of bipolar spectrum disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64:543–52. Erratum. 2007. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64:1039CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Merikangas KR, Cui L, Kattan G, et al.. 2012. Mania with and without depression in a community sample of US adolescents. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69:943–51CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Merikangas KR, Jin R, He JP, et al.. 2011. Prevalence and correlates of bipolar spectrum disorder in the World Mental Health Survey Initiative. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68:241–51CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Mesman E, Nolen WA, Reichart CG, et al.. 2013. The Dutch bipolar offspring study: 12-year follow-up. Am. J. Psychiatry 170:542–49CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Meyer SE, Carlson GA, Wiggs EA, et al.. 2004. A prospective study of the association among impaired executive functioning, childhood attentional problems, and the development of bipolar disorder. Dev. Psychopathol. 16:461–76CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Mick E, Spencer T, Wozniak J, et al.. 2005. Heterogeneity of irritability in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder subjects with and without mood disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 58:576–82CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Molina BS, Hinshaw SP, Swanson JM, et al.. 2009. The MTA at 8 years: prospective follow-up of children treated for combined-type ADHD in a multisite study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 48:484–500CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Moreno C, Laje G, Blanco C, et al.. 2007. National trends in the outpatient diagnosis and treatment of bipolar disorder in youth. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64:1032–39CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Morrison J, Winokur G, Crowe R, et al.. 1973. The Iowa 500. The first follow-up. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 29:678–82CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Natl. Inst. Alcohol Abuse Alcohol. 2001–2005. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Online database. https://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/arh29–2/74-78.htmGoogle Scholar

Natl. Inst. Ment. Health. 2001. National Institute of Mental Health research round table on prepubertal bipolar disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40:871–78CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Nurnberger JI Jr, McInnis M, Reich W, et al.. 2011. A high-risk study of bipolar disorder. Childhood clinical phenotypes as precursors of major mood disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68:1012–20CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Orvaschel H. 1994. Schedule for Affective Disorder and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children: Epidemiologic Version. Ft. Lauderdale, FL: Nova Southeast. Univ., Cent. Psychol. Stud.Google Scholar

Pataki C, Carlson GA. 2013. The comorbidity of ADHD and bipolar disorder: any less confusion? Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 15:372–79CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Perlis RH, Miyahara S, Marangell LB, et al.. 2004. STEP-BD investigators. Long-term implications of early onset in bipolar disorder: data from the first 1000 participants in the Systematic Treatment Enhancement Program for Bipolar Disorder (STEP-BD). Biol. Psychiatry 55:875–78CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Petty CR, Monuteaux MC, Mick E, et al.. 2009. Parsing the familiality of oppositional defiant disorder from that of conduct disorder: a familial risk analysis. J. Psychiatr. Res. 43:345–52CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, et al.. 2007. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am. J. Psychiatry 164:942–48CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Sachs GS, Baldassano CF, Truman CJ, et al.. 2000. Comorbidity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with early- and late-onset bipolar disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 157:466–68CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Shaffer D, Fisher P, Lucas CP, et al.. 2000. NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39:28–38CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Shaw JA, Egeland JA, Endicott J, et al.. 2005. A 10-year prospective study of prodromal patterns for bipolar disorder among Amish youth. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 44:1104–11CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Singh MK, DelBello MP, Stanford KE, et al.. 2007. Psychopathology in children of bipolar parents. J. Affect. Disord. 102:131–36CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Smalley SL, McGough JJ, Moilanen IK, et al.. 2007. Prevalence and psychiatric comorbidity of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in an adolescent Finnish population. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 46:1575–83CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Sobanski E, Banaschewski T, Asherson P, et al.. 2010. Emotional lability in children and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): clinical correlates and familial prevalence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 51:915–23CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Solomon DA, Leon AC, Coryell WH, et al.. 2010. Longitudinal course of bipolar I disorder: duration of mood episodes. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67:339–47CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Srinath S, Janardhan Reddy YC, Girimaji SR, et al.. 1998. A prospective study of bipolar disorder in children and adolescents from India. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 98:437–42CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Stephens JH, McHugh PR. 1991. Chacteristics and long-term follow-up of patients hospitalized for mood disorders in the Phipps Clinic, 1913–1940. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 179:64–73CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Stringaris A, Goodman R. 2009. Mood lability and psychopathology in youth. Psychol. Med. 39:1237–45CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Stringaris A, Santosh P, Leibenluft E, et al.. 2010. Youth meeting symptom and impairment criteria for mania-like episodes lasting less than four days: an epidemiological enquiry. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 51:31–38CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Strober M, Schmidt-Lackner S, Freeman R, et al.. 1995. Recovery and relapse in adolescents with bipolar affective illness: a five-year naturalistic, prospective follow-up. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 34:724–31CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Tillman R, Geller B. 2006. Controlled study of switching from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder to a prepubertal and early adolescent bipolar I disorder phenotype during 6-year prospective follow-up: rate, risk, and predictors. Dev. Psychopathol. 18:1037–53CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Tsuang MT, Dempsey GM. 1979. Long-term outcome of major psychoses. II. Schizoaffective disorder compared with schizophrenia, affective disorders, and a surgical control group. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 36:1302–4CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Van Meter AR, Moreira AL, Youngstrom EA. 2011. Meta-analysis of epidemiologic studies of pediatric bipolar disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry 72:1250–56CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Verhulst FC, van der Ende J, Ferdinand RF, et al.. 1997. The prevalence of DSM-III-R diagnoses in a national sample of Dutch adolescents. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 54:329–36CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Wagner KD, Kowatch RA, Emslie GJ, et al.. 2006. A double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial of oxcarbazepine in the treatment of bipolar disorder in children and adolescents. Am. J. Psychiatry 63:1179–86CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Wagner KD, Redden L, Kowatch RA, et al.. 2009. A double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial of divalproex extended-release in the treatment of bipolar disorder in children and adolescents. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 48:519–32CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Wals M, Hillegers MH, Reichart CG, et al.. 2001. Prevalence of psychopathology in children of a bipolar parent. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40:1094–102CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Weinberg WA, Brumback RA. 1976. Mania in childhood: case studies and a literature review. Am. J. Disord. Child. 130:380–85CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Weinberg WA, Harper CR, Schraufnagel CD, et al.. 1997. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a disease or a symptom complex? J. Pediatr. 130:665–69CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Weiss G, Hechtman L, Milroy T, et al.. 1985. Psychiatric status of hyperactives as adults: a controlled prospective 15-year follow-up of 63 hyperactive children. J. Am. Acad. Child Psychiatry 24:211–20CrossrefGoogle Scholar

West SA, McElroy SL, Strakowski SM, et al.. 1995. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adolescent mania. Am. J. Psychiatry 152:271–73CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Wingo AP, Ghaemi SN. 2007. A systematic review of rates and diagnostic validity of comorbid adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and bipolar disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry 68:1776–84CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Winokur G, Coryell W, Endicott J, et al.. 1993. Further distinctions between manic-depressive illness (bipolar disorder) and primary depressive disorder (unipolar depression). Am. J. Psychiatry 150:1176–81CrossrefGoogle Scholar

World Health Organ. 1992. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. Geneva: WHOGoogle Scholar

Wozniak J, Biederman J, Kiely K, et al.. 1995. Mania-like symptoms suggestive of childhood-onset bipolar disorder in clinically referred children. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 34:867–76CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Wozniak J, Monuteaux M, Richards J, et al.. 2003. Convergence between structured diagnostic interviews and clinical assessment on the diagnosis of pediatric-onset mania. Biol. Psychiatry 53:938–44CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Wozniak J, Petty CR, Schreck M, et al.. 2011. High level of persistence of pediatric bipolar-I disorder from childhood onto adolescent years: a four year prospective longitudinal follow-up study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 45:1273–82CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Youngstrom EA, Findling RL, Calabrese JR. 2003. Who are the comorbid adolescents? Agreement between psychiatric diagnosis, parent, teacher, and youth report. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 31:231–45CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Youngstrom EA, Frazier T, Demeter C, et al.. 2008. Developing a 10-item mania scale from the Parent General Behavior Inventory for children and adolescents. J. Clin. Psychiatry 69:831–39CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Youngstrom EA, Meyers O, Youngstrom JK, et al.. 2006. Diagnostic and measurement issues in the assessment of pediatric bipolar disorder: implications for understanding mood disorder across the life cycle. Dev. Psychopathol. 18:989–1021CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Zahn-Waxler C, Mayfield A, Radke-Yarrow M, et al.. 1988. A follow-up investigation of offspring of parents with bipolar disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 145:506–9CrossrefGoogle Scholar